Skip to main content

Addiction and the Individual

  • Chapter
Addiction

Abstract

Alcohol addiction has been referred to in print as a disease since the end of the 18th century.1 By 1956, the American Medical Association (AMA) had endorsed the disease model, and the view has since received public and unequivocal endorsement by numerous influential medical organizations, including the World Health Organization.2 Researchers commonly use a variety of methodologies that rely on the disease model to attempt to understand various aspects of addiction, and several methods that have been used to study addiction suggest that the disease model is a good one. For instance, twin studies and adoption studies have provided evidence that genetics plays some role in addiction. Moreover, molecular biology explains certain phenomena that occur in the brain with repeated exposure to addictive drugs. These findings seem to make sense of the compulsive element that is associated with many cases of addiction, at the very least suggesting that something other than simple choice is involved. Nevertheless, much care must be taken to situate the cart relative to the horse in this research, and to disentangle the many and diverse threads of addiction research, if we are to evaluate the contention that addiction is a disease.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 89.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Notes

  1. It was so characterized by T. Trotter in 1788. Reproduced in An Essay, Medical, Philosophical, and Chemical on Drunkenness and its Effects on the Human Body, ed. R. Porter (London: Routledge, 1988), and by B. Rush, an American physician, in an 1808 book, An Inquiry into the Effects of Ardent Spirits upon the Human Body and Mind: With an Account of the Means of Preventing, and of the Remedies for Curing Them (Philadelphia: Thomas Dobson, 1808), according to

    Google Scholar 

  2. M. Valverde, in Diseases of the Will: Alcohol and the Dilemmas of Freedom (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1998), 2. According to the Baldwin Research Institute, Rush also believed that dishonesty, political dissent, and being African American were diseases.

    Google Scholar 

  3. George F. Koob and Nora D. Volkow, “Neurocircuitry of Addiction,” Neuropsychopharmacology 35 (2010): 217–138. doi:10.1038/npp.2009.110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. As we will see below, it is not just the amount of pleasure that one experiences that provokes repetition of use or gambling but also the pleasure that one anticipates. See Hans C. Breiter, Itzhak Aharon, Daniel Kahneman, Anders Dale, and Peter Shizgal, “Functional Imaging of Neural Responses to Expectancy and Experience of Monetary Gains and Losses,” Neuron 30 (May 2001): 619–639. In some cases, anticipation seems to play an even more provocative role. In some problem gamblers, almost winning may lead them to gamble even more than winning does. See

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Henry W. Chase and Luke Clark, “Near-win Situations May Encourage Problem Gamblers to Gamble More,” Journal of Neuroscience 30, no. 18 (2010): 6180–6187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. R. L. Solomon and J. D. Corbit, “An Opponent-Process Theory of Motivation. II. Cigarette Addiction,” Journal of Abnormal Psychology 81 (1983): 158–171;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. R. L. Solomon, “Addiction: An Opponent-Process Theory of Acquired Motivation: The Affective Dynamics of Addiction,” in Psychopathology: Experimental Models, ed. J. D. Maser (San Francisco: Freeman, 1977), 66–103.

    Google Scholar 

  8. T. E. Robinson and K. C. Berridge, “The Neural Basis of Drug Craving: An Incentive-Sensitization Theory of Addiction” Brain Research Brain Research Reviews 18 (3) (1993): 247–291;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Robinson and Berridge, “The Psychology and Neurobiology of Addiction: An Incentive-Sensitization View,” Addiction 95, Supplement 2 (2000): S91–117;

    Google Scholar 

  10. Robinson and Berridge, “Review. The Incentive Sensitization Theory of Addiction: Some Current Issues,” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 363, no. 1507 (2008): 3137–3146;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. K. C. Berridge, “Pleasure, Pain, Desire and Dread: Hidden Core Processes of Emotion,” in Well Being: The Foundations of Hedonic Psychology, ed. D. Kahneman, E. Diener, and N. Schwarz (New York: Sage Foundation: 1999), 527–559.

    Google Scholar 

  12. K. C. Berridge and Terry E. Robinson, “Drug Addiction as Incentive Sensitization,” in Addiction and Responsibility, ed. Jeffrey Poland and George Graham (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2011), 21–53.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  13. Clayton Hickey Leonardo Chelazzi, and Jan Theeuwes, “Reward Changes Salience in Human Vision via the Anterior Cingulate,” Journal of Neuroscience 30, no. 33 (2010): 30(33), 11096–11103 suggests that incentive sensitization is achieved through the dopamine reward system.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. A.J. Tindell, K. C. Berridge, J. Zhang, S. Pecifia, and J. W. Aldridge, “Ventral Palladal Neurons Code Incentive Motivation: Amplification by Mesolimbic Sensitization and Amphetamine,” European Journal of Neuroscience 22, no. 10 (2005): 2617–2634;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. P. Vezina, “Sensitization of Midbrain Dopamine Neuron Reactivity and the Sell-Administration of Psychomotor Stimulant Drugs,” Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 27, no. 8 (2004): 827–839;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. N. D. Volkow, G.J. Wang, F. Telang, J. S. Fowler, J. Logan, A. R. Childress et al., “Cocaine Cues and Dopamine in Dorsal Striatum: Mechanism of Craving in Cocaine Addiction, Journal of Neuroscience 26, no. 24 (2006): 6583–6588;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. C. L. Wyvell and K. C. Berridge, “Intra-accumbens and Amphetamine Increases the Conditioned Incentive Salience of Sucrose Reward: Enhancement of Reward ‘Wanting’ without Enhanced ‘Liking’ or Response Reinforcement,” Journal of Neuroscience 20, no. 21 (2000): 8122–8130.

    Google Scholar 

  18. B.J. Everitt, A. Dickinson, and T. W. Robbins, “The Neuropsychological Basis of Addictive Behavior,” Brain Research Review (2001) 36: 129–138;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. J. D. Berke and S. E. Hyman, “Addiction, Dopamine, and the Molecular Mechanisms of Memory,” Neuron 25 (2000): 515–532;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. T. W Robbins and B. J. Everitt, “Drug Addiction: Bad Habits Add Up,” Nature 398 (1999): 567–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. G. D. Logan and W. Cowan, “On the Ability to Inhibit Thought and Action: A Theory of an Act of Control,” Psychological Review 91 (1984): 295–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Daniel Wegner, “Who Is the controller of Controlled Processes?,” The New Unconscious, ed. Ran R. Hassin, James S. Uleman, and John A. Bargh (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2005), 19–37.

    Google Scholar 

  23. John A. Bargh, ed. Social Psychology and the Unconscious: The Automaticity of Higher Mental Processes (New York: Psychology Press, 2007);

    Google Scholar 

  24. David D. Franks, Neurosociology: The Nexus Between Neuroscience and Social Psychology (New York: Springer, 2010), Chapter 4.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  25. D. B. Newlin and K. A. Strubler, “The Habitual Brain: An ‘Adapted Habit’ Theory of Substance Use Disorders,” Substance Use and Misuse 42, no. 2–3 (2007): 503–526;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. George Messinis, “Habit Formation and the Theory of Addiction,” Journal of Economic Surveys 13, no. 4 (2009): 417–442. doi: 10.1111/1467–6419.00089.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Daniel J. Siegal, The Developing Mind (New York: Guilford Press, 1999), 28–32.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Reinout W. Wiers and Alan W. Stacy, “Implicit Cognition and Addiction,” Current Directions in Psychological Science 16, no. 6 (Dec. 2006): 292–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. A. E. Kelley and K. C. Berridge, “The Neuroscience of Natural Rewards: Relevance to Addictive Drugs,” Journal of Neuroscience 22 (2002): 3306–3311.

    Google Scholar 

  30. A. E. Kelley and K. C. Berridge, “The Neuroscience of Natural Rewards: Relevance to Addictive Drugs,” Journal of Neuroscience 22 (2002): 3306–3311;

    Google Scholar 

  31. Steven E. Hyman, Robert C. Malenka, and Eric J. Nestler, “Addiction: The Role of Reward-Related Learning and Memory,” Annual Review of Neuroscience 29 (2006): 565–598; Koob and Volkow (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. But even this is not without dispute, as some researchers have argued that more than one “type” of alcoholism exist, and require independent discussion. One sort, it is argued, the susceptibility to lose control alter drinking begins, is genetically distinct from the susceptibility to lose control after drinking begins, and requires a different analysis. While the first kind of alcoholism may be accounted for in terms of dopamine and hedonic experiences, the other, characterized by impulse control problems, is attributed to a dysfunc-tion in serotonin regulation. Both of these “types” of alcoholism, however, are seen as the result of the interaction of genetic and environmental lactors. See C. R. Cloninger, “Neurogenetic Adaptive Mechanisms in Alcoholism,” Science 236 (1987): 410–416; also

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Christina S. Barr, Melanie L. Schwandt, Timothy K. Newman, and J. Dee Higley, “The Use of Adolescent Nonhuman Primates to Model Human Alcohol Intake: Neurobiological, Genetic, and Psychological Variables,” Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1021 (2004): 221–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. N. D. Volkow, G. J. Want, J. S. Fowler, S. J. Gatley, Y. S. Ding, J. Logan et al., “Relationship between Psychostimulant-induced ‘High’ and Dopamine Transporter Occupancy,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 93 (1996): 10388–10392;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. N. D. Volkow and J. M. Swanson, “Variables That Affect the Clinical Use and Abuse of Methylphenidate in the Treatment of ADHD,” American Journal of Psychiatry 160 (2003): 1909–1918;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. A. A. Grace, “The Tonic/Phasic Model of Dopamine System Regulation and Its Implications for Understanding Alcohol and Psychostimulant Craving,” Addiction 95, Suppl. 2 (2000): S119–S128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. T. E. Robinson and K.C. Berridge, “Addiction,” Annual Review of Psychology 54 (2003): 25–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. T. E. Robinson and B. Kolb, “Alterations in the Morphology of Dendrites and Dendritic Spines in the Nucleus Accombens and Pre-frontal Cortex Following Repeated Treatment with Amphetamine or Cocaine,” European Journal of Neuroscience 11 (1999): 1598–1604;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. G. F. Koob, S. B. Caine, L. Parsons, A. Markous, and F. Weiss, “Opponent Process Model and Psychostimulant Addiction,” Pharmacological Biochemistry and Behavior 57 (1997): 513–521;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. G. F. Koob and M. Le Moal, “Drug Abuse: Hedonic Homeostatic Dysregulation,” Science 278 (1997): 52–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Wolfram Schultz, Peter Dayan, and P. Read Montague, “A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward,” Science 275, no. 5306 (1997): 1593–1599;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. K. A. Hadland, M. F. S. Rushworth, D. Gaffan, and R. E. Passingham, “The Anterior Cingulate and Reward-Guided Selection of Actions,” Journal of Neurophysiology 89, no. 2 (2003): 1161–1164;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Celine Amiez, Jean-Paul Joseph, and Emmanuel Procyk, “Anterior Cingulate Error-related Activity Is Modulated by Predicted Reward,” European Journal of Neuroscience 21 (2005): 3447–3452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. B. J. Everitt and T. W. Robbins, “Neural Systems of Reinforcement for Drug Addiction: From Actions to Habits to Compulsion,” Nature Neuroscience 8 (2005): 1481–1489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Terry E. Robinson and Kent C. Berridge, “Addiction,” Annual Review of Psychology 54 (2003): 25–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. W. D. Yao, R. R. Gainetdinov, M. I. Arbuckle, T. D. Sotnikova, M. Cyr, J. M Beaulieu et al., “Identification of PSD-95 as a Regulator of Dopamine-Mediated Synaptic and Behavioral Plasticity,” Neuron 41 (1004): 625–638.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. N. D. Volkow, J. S. Fowler, G. J. Wang, and J. M. Swanson, “Dopamine in Drug Abuse and Addiction: Results from Imaging Studies and Treatment Implications,” Molecular Psychiatry 9 (2004): 557–569.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Martin Sarter, Gary G. Berntson, and John Cacioppo, “Brain Imaging and Cognitive Neuroscience: Toward Strong Inference in Attributing Function to Structure,” American Psychologist 51, no. 1 (1996): 13–21;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Karl Friston, “Beyond Phrenology: What Can Neuroimaging Tell Us about Distributed Circuitry?,” Annual Review of Neuroscience 25 (2002): 221–250. doi:10.1146//annurev.neuro.25.112701.142846.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Donald B. Douglas, “Alcoholism as an Addiction: The Disease Concept Reconsidered,” Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 3, no. 2 (1986): 115–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. N. D. Volkow, G. J. Want, J. S. Fowler, S. J. Gatley, Y. S. Ding, J. Logan et al., “Relationship between Psychostimulant-induced ‘High’ and Dopamine Transporter Occupancy,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 93 (1996): 10388–10392;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. N. D. Volkow and J. M. Swanson, “Variables That Affect the Clinical Use and Abuse of Methylphenidate in the Treatment of ADHD,” American Journal of Psychiatry 160 (2003): 1909–1918;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. A. A. Grace, “The Tonic/Phasic Model of Dopamine System Regulation and Its Implications for Understanding Alcohol and Psychostimulant Craving,” Addiction 95, Suppl. 2 (2000): S119–S128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Stanton Peel, “Denial — of Reality and of Freedom — in Addiction Research and Treatment,” Bulletin of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors 5, no. 4 (1986): 149–166.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Mark R. Hutchinson et al., “Opioid Activation of Toll-Like Receptor 4 Contributes to Drug Reinforcement,” Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 32, no. 33 (2012): 11187–11200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Stanton Peele, Diseasing of America: Addiction Treatment Out of Control (Lexington, MA: Lexington Books, 1989).

    Google Scholar 

  57. George Ainslie, Breakdown of Will (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2001); also

    Book  Google Scholar 

  58. Ainslie, “The Core Process in Addictions and Other Impulses: Hyperbolic Discounting Versus Conditioning and Framing,” in What Is Addiction? eds D. Ross, H. Kincaid, D. Spurrett, and P. Collins (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2009), 211–245.

    Google Scholar 

  59. George Ainslie, “Beyond Microeconomics: Conflict Among Interests in a Multiple Self as a Determinant of Value” in The Multiple Self, ed. John Elster (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1986), 133–175.

    Google Scholar 

  60. George Anslie and V. Haendel, “The Motives of the Will,” in Etiology Aspects of Alcohol and Drug Abuse, ed. E. Gottheil, K. Druley T. Skodals, and H. Waxman (Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas, 1983), 119–140;

    Google Scholar 

  61. W. K. Bickel, A. L. Odum, and G. J. Madden, “Impulsivity and Cigarette Smoking: Delay Discounting in Current, Never, and Ex-smokers,” Psychopharmacology 146, no. 4 (1999): 447–454;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. A. L. Bretteville-Jensen, “Addiction and Discounting” Journal of Health Economics 18, no. 4 (1999): 393–407;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. V. R. Fuchs, “Time Preferences and Health: An Exploratory Study,” in Economic Aspects of Health, ed. V. R. Fuchs (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1956), 92–120);

    Google Scholar 

  64. K. N. Kirby, N. M Petry and W. K. Bickel, “Heroin Addicts Have Higher Discount Rates for Delayed Rewards Than Non-drug-using Controls,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 128, no. 1 (1999): 78–87;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. G. J. Madden, N. M. Petry, G.J. Badger, and W. K. Bickel, “Impulsive and Self-control Choices in Opioid-dependent Patients and Non-drug-using Control Participants: Drug and Monetary Rewards,” Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 5, no. 3 (1997): 256–262;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. S. Mitchell, “Measures of Impulsivity in Cigarette Smokers and Non-Smokers Psychopharmacology 146, no. 4 (1999): 455–464; J. Monterosso, G. Ainslie, J. Xu, X. Cordova, C. P. Domier, and E. D. London “Frontoparietal Cortical Activity of Methamphetamine-dependent and Comparison Subjects Performing a Delay Discounting Task,” Human Brain Mapping 28, no. 5 (2007): 383–393;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. R. E. Vuchinich, “Hyperbolic Temporal Discounting in Social Drinkers and Problem Drinkers,” Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 6, no. 3 (1998): 292–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. George Ainslie, “Free Will as Recursive Self-Prediction,” in Addiction and Responsibility, ed. Jeffrey Poland and George Graham (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2011), 64.

    Google Scholar 

  69. Mark Muraven and Elisaveta Slessareva, “Mechanisms of Self-Control Failure: Motivation and Limited Resources,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 29 (2003): 894–906;

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Mark Muraven, Dianne M. Tice, and Roy F. Baumeister, “Self-Control as a Limited Resource: Regulatory Depletion Patterns,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74 (1998): 774–789.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Kathleen D. Vohs and Todd F. Heatherton, “Self-Regulatory Failure: A Resource-Depletion Approach,” Psychological Science 11, no. 3 (2000): 249–254; also see

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. D. Kahn, J. Polivy and C. P. Herman, “Conformity and Dietary Disinhibition: A Test of the Ego Strength Model of Self-Regulation,” International Journal of Eating Disorders 33, no. 2 (2003): 165–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Neil Levey, “Addiction, Responsibility, and Ego Depletion,” in Addiction and Responsibility, ed. Jeffrey Poland and George Graham (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2011), 101–111.

    Google Scholar 

  74. G. Loewenstein, “Willpower: A Decision Theorist’s Perspective,” Law and Philosophy 19, no. 1 (2000): 51–76.

    Google Scholar 

  75. James MacKillop and Christopher W. Kahler, “Delayed Reward Discounting Predicts Treatment Response for Heavy Drinkers Receiving Smoking Cessation Treatment,” Drug and Alcohol Dependence 104, no. 3 (2009): 197–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Bruce D. Perry, “Childhood Experience and the Expression of Genetic Potential: What Childhood Neglect Tells Us about Nature and Nurture,” Brain and Mind 3, no. 1 (2002): 79–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Copyright information

© 2016 Candice L. Shelby

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Shelby, C.L. (2016). Addiction and the Individual. In: Addiction. Palgrave Macmillan, London. https://doi.org/10.1057/9781137552853_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics