Skip to main content

Computation

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Cognitive Mechanisms of Belief Change
  • 824 Accesses

Abstract

This chapter presents the core of the belief change model, or the engine of belief. It is here where the heavy lifting gets done at the confluence of the mind and contextual forces. Conversion to a new suite of beliefs works in a multifaceted way; a dynamic event where the cognitive transition is mediated by structural and contextual forces, along with the baggage of previous experiences mixed with existing concepts. The engine of conversion revolves around a series of seven mechanisms, which collectively operationalise the engine into defined practices, behaviours, and thinking patterns. From inputs that land in the mind, computation spits outs inferences automatically, some of which we act upon immediately, while others allow us the opportunity to reflect before making a decision to act. As a result, we experience strong leanings towards certain kinds of thinking. In addition, all of our senses deliver information pertinent to belief change because they deliver embodied expressions of emotional states, which after repeated exposures, act to stimulate the very emotions they signify. Not only are emotional experiences during belief-related activities relevant, but so too are their role in linking action, thought, and feeling through ingrained pathways that play out like inexorable tracks once the belief stimulus is activated. The model also suggests that rituals play a pivotal role not just in the transmission of belief concepts but also in the process of attuning cognition for ongoing receptivity to deeper, more elaborate concepts.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 69.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 89.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Adolphs, R. 2002. Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 12: 169–177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adolphs, R., D. Tranel, and A.R. Damasio. 1998. The human amygdala in social judgment. Nature 393: 470–474.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aftanas, L.I., and S.A. Golocheikine. 2001. Human anterior and frontal midline theta and lower alpha reflect emotionally positive state and internalized attention: High-resolution EEG investigation of meditation. Neuroscience Letters 310(1): 57–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Albert, S., and D.A. Whetten. 1985. Organizational identity. Research in Organizational Behavior 7: 263–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alvard, M. 2003. Kinship, lineage identity, and an evolutionary perspective on the structure of cooperative big game hunting groups in Indonesia. Human Nature 14: 129–163.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Andreoni, J., and A. Sanchez. 2014. Do beliefs justify actions or do actions justify beliefs? An experiment on stated beliefs, revealed beliefs, and social-image manipulation (No. w20649). Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Appiah, K.A. 2009. Explaining religion: Notes toward a research agenda. In Games, groups, and the global good, ed. S.A. Levin, 195–203. Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Arkes, H.R., and C. Blumer. 1985. The psychology of sunk cost. Organization Behavior and Human Decision Process 35: 124–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, F., G. Isen, M. Alice, and U. Turken. 1999. A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review 106(3): 529–550.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Atran, S. 2002. In Gods we trust: The evolutionary landscape of religion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Austin, J. 1998. Zen and the brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Azari, N.P., and D. Birnbacher. 2004. The role of cognition and feeling in religious experience. Zygon 39(4): 901–918.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Azari, N.P., J. Nickel, G. Wunderlich, M. Niedeggen, H. Hefter, L. Tellmann, H. Herzog, P. Stoerig, D. Birnbacher, and R.J. Sietz. 2001. Neurocorrelates of religious experience. European Journal of Neuroscience 13(8): 1649–1652.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, K., and N.J.S. Gibson. 2013. Supernatural agency: Individual difference predictors and situational correlates. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 23(1): 42–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, J.L. 2004. Why would anyone believe in god? Walnut Creek: AltaMira Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L.W., A.K. Barbey, W.K. Simmons, and A. Santos. 2005. Embodiment in religious knowledge. Journal of Cognition and Culture 5(1-2): 14–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beauvois, J.L., and N. Dubois. 2000. Affordances in social judgment: Experimental proof of why it is a mistake to ignore how others behave towards a target and look solely at how the target behaves. Swiss Journal of Psychology/Schweizerische Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Revue Suisse de Psychologie 59(1): 16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bell, V., E. Grech, C. Maiden, P.W. Halligan, and H.D. Ellis. 2005. ‘Internet delusions’: A case series and theoretical integration. Psychopathology 38(3): 144–150.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bering, J. 2009. Atheism is only skin deep: Geertz and Markússon rely mistakenly on sociodemographic data as meaningful indicators of underlying cognition. Religion 40(3): 166–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bering, J.M., and D.D. Johnson. 2005. “O Lord you perceive my thoughts from afar”: Recursiveness and the evolution of supernatural agency. Journal of Cognition and Culture 5: 1–2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berna, F., C. Huron, M. Kazes, I. Offerlin-Meyer, D. Willard, P. Verry, G. Hedelin, M.O. Krebs, and J.M. Danion. 2013. Chronic persecutory delusion and autobiographical memories in patients with schizophrenia: A diary study. The Israel Journal of Psychiatry and Related Sciences 51(1): 25–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bird, R.B., and E.A. Smith. 2005. Signaling theory, strategic interaction, and symbolic capital. Current Anthropology 46(2): 221–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bogdan, R.J. 2005. Pretending as imaginative rehearsal for cultural conformity. Journal of Cognition and Culture 5(1–2): 191–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Booth, J.N., S.A. Koren, and M.A. Persinger. 2003. Increased proportions of sensed presences and occipital spikes with 1- and 10-msec. point duration of continuous 7-Hz transcerebral magnetic fields. Perceptual and Motor Skills 97(3 Pt 1): 951–952.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boudry, M., and J. Braeckman. 2012. How convenient! The epistemic rationale of self-validating belief systems. Philosophical Psychology 25(3): 341–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P. 2001. Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P. 2003. Religious thought and behaviour as by-products of brain function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7(3): 119–124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P., and P. Lienard. 2006. Why ritualized behavior? Precaution systems and action parsing in developmental, pathological and cultural rituals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29: 595–613.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradford, T.W., and J.F. Sherry. 2015. Domesticating public space through ritual: Tailgating as vestaval. Journal of Consumer Research 42(1): 130–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brumann, C. 2001. Religious consensus and secular dissent: Two alternative paths to survival for utopian communes. Anthropos 96: 87–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bulbulia, J. 2009. Religiosity as mental time travel: Cognitive adaptations for religious behavior. In The believing primate: Scientific, philosophical and theological perspectives on the evolution of religion, ed. J. Schloss and M. Murray, 44–75. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Burzik, A. 2004. On the neurophysiology of flow thought-provoking studies from sport and music psychology, neurofeedback and trance research. Paper read at 2nd European Conference on Positive Psychology, 5–8 July, at Verbania Pallanza, Italy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, N. 2001. Physiology of behavior, 7th ed. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carter, O., D. Presti, C. Callistemon, Y. Ungerer, G. Liu, and J. Pettigrew. 2004. Meditation alters perceptual rivalry in Tibetan Buddhist monks. Current Biology 15(11): R412–R413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Choi, I., R.E. Nisbett, and A. Norenzayan. 1999. Causal attribution across cultures: Variation and universality. Psychological Bulletin 125: 47–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cicourel, A.V. 2012. Origin and demise of socio-cultural presentations of self from birth to death: Caregiver ‘scaffolding’ practices necessary for guiding and sustaining communal social structure throughout the life cycle. Sociology 47(1): 51–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, A.B., and P. Rozin. 2001. Religion and the morality of mentality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81(4): 697–710.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., R. Langdon, and R. McKay. 2011. Delusional belief. Annual Review of Psychology 62: 271–298.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Colzato, L.S., I. van Beest, W.P. van den Wildenberg, C. Scorolli, S. Dorchin, N. Meiran, A.M. Borghi, and B. Hommel. 2010. God: Do I have your attention? Cognition 117(1): 87–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. 1990. Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cysarz, D., and A. Bussing. 2005. Cardiorespiratory synchronization during Zen meditation. European Journal of Applied Physiology 95(1): 88–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • D’Aquili, E., and A. Newberg. 1998. The neuropsychological basis of religions, or why god won’t go away. Zygon 33(2): 187–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. 1999. The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York: Harcourt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. 2001. Emotion and the human brain. Journal of the Annual New York Academy of Science 935(1): 101–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R.J., J. Kabat-Zinn, J. Schumacher, M. Rosenkranz, D. Muller, S.F. Santorelli, F. Urbanowski, A. Harrington, K. Bonus, and J.F. Sheridan. 2003. Alterations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation. Psychosomatic Medicine 65(4): 564–570.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, P.M., and L.R. Rambo. 2005. Converting: Toward a cognitive theory of religious change. In Soul, psyche, brain: New directions in the study of religion and brain-mind science, ed. K. Bulkeley, 159–173. New York: Palgrave, Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Decety, J. 1996. Do imagined and executed actions share the same neural substrate? Cognitive Brain Research 3(2): 87–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Delgado, M.R. 2007. Reward-related response in the human striatum. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1104: 70–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. 2006. Breaking the spell: Religion as a natural phenomenon. New York: Viking.

    Google Scholar 

  • di Pellegrino, G., L. Fadiga, L. Fogassi, V. Gallese, and G. Rizzolatti. 1992. Understanding motor events: A neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research 91: 176–180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diakidoy, I.A.N., S.A. Christodoulou, G. Floros, K. Iordanou, and P.V. Kargopoulos. 2015. Forming a belief: The contribution of comprehension to the evaluation and persuasive impact of argumentative text. British Journal of Educational Psychology. doi:10.1111/bjep.12074. Article first published online: 30 Mar 2015.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dietrich, A. 2003. Functional neuroanatomy of altered states of consciousness: The transient hypofrontality hypothesis. Consciousness and Cognition 12: 231–256.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Donahoe, J.W., and D.C. Palmer. 1993. Learning and complex behavior. New York: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dow, J.W. 2006. The evolution of religion: Three anthropological approaches. Method and Theory in the Study of Religion 18(1): 67–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dulaney, S., and A. Fiske. 1994. Cultural rituals and obsessive-compulsive disorder: Is there a common psychological mechanism? Ethos 22(3): 243–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Durkheim, E. 1961[1915]. The Elementary Forms of the Religious Life. Trans. J.W. Swain. New York: Collier Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dutton, J.E., and J.M. Dukerich. 1991. Keeping an eye on the mirror: Image and identity in organizational adaptation. Academy of Management Journal 34(3): 517–557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, S. 1998. Cognitive-experiential self theory. In Advanced personality, ed. D.F. Barone, M. Hersen, and V.B. Van Hasselt, 211–238. New York: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R.H. 2001. On the automatic activation of associated evaluations: An overview. Cognition and Emotion 15: 115–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernback, J. 2007. Beyond the diluted community concept: A symbolic interactionist perspective on online social relations. New Media & Society 9(1): 49–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finke, R., and S. Rodney. 1992. The Churching of America, 1776–1990: Winners and losers in our religious economy. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, D. 2007. Suspicious minds: The psychology of persecutory delusions. Clinical Psychology Review 27: 425–457.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Funder, D.C. 1987. Errors and mistakes: Evaluating the accuracy of social judgment. Psychological Bulletin 101: 75–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fuster, J. 2003. Cortex and mind: Unifying cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garety, P.A., and D. Freeman. 2013. The past and future of delusions research: From the inexplicable to the treatable. The British Journal of Psychiatry 203(5): 327–333.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga, L., R. Ivry, and G. Mangun. 2002. Cognitive neuroscience: The biology of the mind, 2nd ed. New York: W.W. Norton & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geertz, C. 2000. Available light. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D.T., E.C. Pinel, T.D. Wilson, S.J. Blumberg, and T.P. Wheatley. 1998. Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 75: 617–638.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. 1967. Interaction rituals. Garden City: Anchor Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A.C., S. Goldman, and P. van den Broek. 2002. Narrative comprehension, causality and coherence. In Narrative impact: Social and cognitive foundations, ed. M. Green, J. Strange, and T. Brock, 93–110. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, S. 2002. Animal animism: Evolutionary roots of religious cognition. In Current approaches in the cognitive science of religion, ed. I. Pyysiäinen and V. Anttonen, 38–67. London: Continuum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, S., S.A. Sheth, and M.S. Cohen. 2008. Functional neuroimaging of belief, disbelief, and uncertainty. Annals of Neurology 63: 141–147.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, S., J.T. Kaplan, A. Curiel, S.Y. Bookheimer, M. Iacoboni, and M. Cohen. 2009. The neural correlates of religious and nonreligious belief. PLoS ONE 4(10): 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haselton, M.G., and D.M. Buss. 2000. Error management theory: A new perspective on biases in cross-sex mind reading. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78: 81–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hatch, M.J., and M. Schultz. 2002. The dynamics of organizational identity. Human Relations 55(08): 989–1018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heberlein, A.S., and R. Adolphs. 2005. Functional anatomy of human social cognition. In The cognitive neuroscience of social behaviour, ed. A. Easton and N. Emery, 157–194. East Sussex: Psychology Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., L.S. Colzato, C. Scorolli, A.M. Borghi, and W.P.M. van den Wildenberg. 2011. Religion and action control: Faith-specific modulation of the Simon effect but not stop-signal performance. Cognition 120: 177–185.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horne, Z., D. Powell, and J. Spino. 2013. Belief updating in moral dilemmas. Review of Philosophy and Psychology 4(4): 705–714.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horne, Z., D. Powell, and J. Hummel. 2015. A single counterexample leads to moral belief revision. Cognitive Science 39: 1950–1964. doi:10.1111/cogs.12223, 1–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horvitz, J.C., T. Stewart, and B.L. Jacobs. 1997. Burst activity of ventral segmental dopamine neurons as elicited by sensory stimuli. Brain Research 759: 251–258.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hsee, C.K., and R. Hastie. 2005. Decision and experience: Why don’t we choose what makes us happy? Trends in Cognitive Science 10(1): 31–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iannaccone, L. 1994. Why strict churches are strong. American Journal of Sociology 99: 1180–1211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Irons, W. 2001. Religion as a hard-to-fake sign of commitment. In Evolution and the capacity for commitment, ed. R. Nesse, 292–309. New York: Russell Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iyengar, S.S., and M. Lepper. 2000. When choice is demotivating: Can one desire too much of a good thing? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 76: 995–1006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, R. 1996. Social identity. London: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jindra, I. 2008. Religious stage development among converts to different religious groups. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 18: 195–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D.P. 2009. The error of God: Error management theory, religion, and the evolution of cooperation. In Games, groups, and the global good, ed. S.A. Levin, 169–180. Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., B.L. Fredrickson, C.A. Schreiber, and D.A. Redelmeier. 1993. When more pain is preferred to less: Adding a better end. Psychological Science 4: 401–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karney, B.R., and R.H. Coombs. 2000. Memory bias in long-term close relationships: Consistency or improvement? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 26: 959–970.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelemen, D. 2006. Are children ‘intuitive theists’?: Reasoning about purpose and design in nature. In Religion and cognition, ed. D.J. Slone, 99–113. London: Equinox Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kidd, C., H. Palmeri, and R.N. Aslin. 2013. Rational snacking: Young children’s decision-making on the marshmallow task is moderated by beliefs about environmental reliability. Cognition 126(1): 109–114.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Koepp, M.J., R.N. Gunn, A.D. Lawrence, V.J. Cunningham, A. Dagher, T. Jones, D.J. Brooks, C.J. Bench, and P.M. Grasby. 1998. Evidence for stiatal dopamine release during a video game. Nature 393: 266–268.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Layard, R. 2005. Happiness: Lessons from a new science. London: Penguin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Legare, C.H. 2014. The contributions of explanation and exploration to children’s scientific reasoning. Child Development Perspectives 8(2): 101–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Legare, C.H., and A.L. Souza. 2014. Searching for control: Priming randomness increases the evaluation of ritual efficacy. Cognitive Science 38(1): 152–161.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lehmann, D., P. Faber, P. Achermann, D. Jeanmonod, R. Gianotti, and D. Pizzagalli. 2001. Brain sources of EEG gamma frequency during volitionally meditation-induced, altered states of consciousness, and experience of the self. Psychiatry Research 108: 111–121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Livingston, K.R. 2005. Religious practice, brain, and belief. Journal of Cognition and Culture 5(1-1): 75–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lumer, E., K. Friston, and G. Rees. 1998. Neural correlates of perceptual rivalry in the human brain. Science 280: 1930–1934.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lutz, A., L.L. Greischar, N.B. Rawlings, M. Ricard, and R.J. Davidson. 2004. Long-term meditators self-induce high-amplitude gamma synchrony during mental practice. PNAS 101(46): 16369–16373.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Makarec, K., and M. Persinger. 1985. Temporal lobe signs: Electroencephalographic validity and enhanced scores in special populations. Perceptual and Motor Skills 60: 831–842.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCauley, R.N. 2011. Why religion is natural and science is not. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCauley, R.N., and E.T. Lawson. 2002. Bringing ritual to mind. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • McClenon, J. 2002. Wondrous healing: Shamanism, human evolution and the origin of religion. DeKalb: Northern Illinois University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McElreath, R., R. Boyd, and P. Richerson. 2003. Shared norms can lead to the evolution of ethnic markers. Current Anthropology 44: 122–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McInnman, A., and J. Grove. 1991. Peak moments in sport: A literature review. Quest 43: 333–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mead, G.H. 1934. Mind, self and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mithen, S.J. 1996. The prehistory of the mind: The cognitive origins of art, religion and science. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, J.S., A. Ohman, and R.J. Dolan. 1998. Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala. Nature 393: 467–470.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morrison, A.P. 2001. The interpretation of intrusions in psychosis: An integrative cognitive approach to hallucinations and delusions. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy 29(03): 257–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newberg, A.B., A. Alavi, M. Baime, M. Pourdehnad, J. Santanna, and E. d’Aquili. 2001a. The measurement of regional cerebral blood for flow during the complex cognitive tasks of meditation: A preliminary SPECT study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging Section 106: 113–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newberg, A.B., E. D’Aquili, and V. Rause. 2001b. Why God won’t go away: Brain science and the biology of belief. New York: Ballantine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norenzayan, A., and S.J. Heine. 2005. Psychological universals across cultures: What are they and how can we know? Psychological Bulletin 135: 763–784.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norenzayan, A., I. Choi, and R.E. Nisbett. 2002. Cultural similarities and differences in social inference: Evidence from behavioral predictions and lay theories of behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 28(1): 109–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norenzayan, A., I. Choi, and K. Perg. 2007. Perception and cognition. In Handbook of cultural psychology, ed. S. Kitayama and D. Cohen, 569–594. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oullier, O., K.J. Jantzen, F.L. Steinberg, and J.A.S. Kelso. 2005. Neural substrates of real and imagined sensorimotor coordination. Cerebral Cortex 15(7): 975–985.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pechey, R., and P. Halligan. 2011. The prevalence of delusion-like beliefs relative to sociocultural beliefs in the general population. Psychopathology 44(2): 106–115.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pechey, R., and P. Halligan. 2012a. Prevalence and correlates of anomalous experiences in a large non-clinical sample. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice 85(2): 150–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pechey, R., and P. Halligan. 2012b. Exploring the folk understanding of belief: Identifying key dimensions endorsed in the general population. Journal of Cognition and Culture 12(1-2): 81–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Persinger, M.A. 2001. The neuropsychiatry of paranormal experiences. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 13(4): 515–524.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Persinger, M.A. 2003. The sensed presence within experimental settings: Implications for the male and female concept of self. Journal of Psychology 137(1): 5–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pfeifer, S. 1999. Demonic attributions in nondelusional disorders. Psychopathology 32(5): 252–259.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pigliucci, M. 2010. Nonsense on stilts: How to tell science from bunk. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. 2002. The blank slate. London: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pyysiäinen, I. 2003. How religion works: Towards a new cognitive science of religion. Boston: Brill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rai, T.S., and A.P. Fiske. 2011. Moral psychology is relationship regulation: Moral motives for unity, hierarchy, equality, and proportionality. Psychological Review 118(1): 57–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran, V.S. 1995. Mirror neurons and imitation learning as the driving force behind ‘the great leap forward’ in human evolution. Paper read at Decade of the Brain Lecture, Silver Jubilee meeting of the Society for Neuroscience.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rambo, L.R. 1999. Theories of conversion: Understanding and interpreting religious change. Social Compass 46: 259–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rappaport, R.A. 1999. Ritual and religion in the making of humanity. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ravasi, D., and J.V. Rekom. 2003. Key issues in organizational identity and identification theory. Corporate Reputation Review 6(2): 118–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richert, R.A., H. Whitehouse, and E. Stewart. 2005. Memory and analogical thinking in high-arousal rituals. In Mind and religion: Psychological and cognitive foundations of religiosity, ed. H. Whitehouse and R.N. McCauley, 127–145. Walnut Creek: AltaMira.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roll, W.G., M.A. Persinger, D.L. Webster, S.G. Tiller, and C.M. Cook. 2002. Neurobehavioral and neurometabolic (SPECT) correlates of paranormal information: Involvement of the right hemisphere and its sensitivity to weak complex magnetic fields. International Journal of Neuroscience 112(2): 197–224.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saucier, G. 2010. The structure of social effects: Personality as impact on others. European Journal of Personality 24(3): 222–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schjoedt, U., H. Stødkilde-Jørgensen, A.W. Geertz, and A. Roepstorff. 2008. Rewarding prayers. Neuroscience Letters 443: 165–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, L., and L. Trainor. 2001. Frontal brain electrical activity (EEG) distinguishes valence and intensity of musical emotions. Cognition and Emotion 15: 487–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, D., V.P. Slaughter, S.I. Becker, and P.E. Dux. 2014. Implicit false-belief processing in the human brain. NeuroImage 101: 268–275.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shariff, A., and A. Norenzayan. 2007. God is watching you: Supernatural agent concepts increase prosocial behavior in an anonymous economic game. Psychological Science 18(9): 803–809.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sorensen, J.S. 2002. The morphology and function of magic revisited. In Current approaches in the cognitive science of religion, ed. I. Pyysiainen and V. Anttonen, 177–202. London: Continuum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sosis, R. 2003. Why aren’t we all Hutterites? Costly signaling theory and religious behavior. Human Relations 14: 91–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sosis, R. 2004. The adaptive value of religious ritual. American Scientist 92: 166–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sosis, R., and C.S. Alcorta. 2003. Signaling, solidarity and the sacred: The evolution of religious behavior. Evolutionary Anthropology 12: 264–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sosis, R., and E. Bressler. 2003. Cooperation and commune longevity: A test of the costly signaling theory of religion. Cross-Cultural Research 37: 211–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sosis, R., and B. Ruffle. 2003. Religious ritual and cooperation: Testing for a relationship on Israeli religious and secular kibbutzim. Current Anthropology 44: 713–722.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sosis, R., and B. Ruffle. 2004. Evolution of cooperation: Field experiments on Israeli Kibbutzim. Socioeconomic Aspects of Human Behavioral Ecology, Research in Economic Anthropology 23: 89–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taleb, N. 2012. Antifragile: Things that gain from disorder. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tambiah, S.J. 1996. Leveling crowds: Ethnonationalist conflicts and collective violence in South Asia. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thagard, P. 2005a. Mind: Introduction to cognitive science, 2nd ed. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thagard, P. 2005b. The emotional coherence of religion. Journal of Cognition and Culture 5(1-2): 58–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tremlin, T. 2005. Divergent religion: A dual-process model of religious thought, behavior, and morphology. In Mind and religion: Psychological and cognitive foundations of religiosity, ed. H. Whitehouse and R.N. McCauley, 69–83. Walnut Creek: AltaMira.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, V. 1969. The ritual process: Structure and anti-structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Boven, L., and G. Loewenstein. 2003. Social projection of transient drive states. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 29(9): 1159–1168.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Boven, L., G. Loewenstein, and D. Dunning. 2005. The illusion of courage in social predictions: Underestimating the impact of few and embarrassment on other people. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 96: 130–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watts, F.N. 1997. Psychological and religious perspectives on emotion. Zygon 32(2): 243–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D.M., B. Sparrow, and L. Winerman. 2004. Vicarious agency: Experiencing control over the movements of others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 86(6): 838–848.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehouse, H. 2004. Modes of religiosity: A cognitive theory of religious transmission. Walnut Creek: Alta Mira Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T.D., and D. Gilbert. 2005. Affective forecasting: Knowing what to want. Current Directions in Psychological Science 14(3): 131–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D.S., D. Near, and R.R. Miller. 1996. Machiavellianism: A synthesis of the evolutionary and psychological literatures. Psychological Bulletin 119: 285–299.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T.D., T.P. Wheatley, J.M. Meyers, D.T. Gilbert, and D. Axsom. 2000. Focalism: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78: 821–836.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winkelman, M. 2002. Shamanism as neurotheology and evolutionary psychology. The American Behavioral Scientist 45(12): 1875–1887.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wise, A. 2002. Awakening the mind. A guide to mastering the power of your brain waves. New York: Tarcher/Putnam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, P.T. 1967. Affective arousal: Some implications. American Psychologist 22: 32–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Copyright information

© 2016 The Editor(s) (if applicable) and The Author(s)

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Smith, A.C.T. (2016). Computation. In: Cognitive Mechanisms of Belief Change. Palgrave Macmillan, London. https://doi.org/10.1057/978-1-137-57895-2_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics