Skip to main content
Articles

Personality, Emotionality, and Risk Prediction

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001.27.3.127

We suggest a new approach for measuring individual differences in optimistic bias that adjusts risk estimates for oneself for: (1) risk estimates for other persons to control for response tendencies, and (2) risk estimates by knowledgeable informants to control for differences in actual risk. In two studies, we measured Positive and Negative Emotionality by self-reports and reports by knowledgeable informants. Moreover, likelihood estimates that pleasant and unpleasant events will occur to oneself and to an average other person were collected, and the knowledgeable informants provided risk estimates for the research participants. Risk estimates by knowledgeable informants were even more optimistic than self-estimates, and optimistic bias was related directly to Positive Emotionality and inversely to Negative Emotionality. These effects of personality on optimistic bias were not mediated by current mood.

References

  • Alloy, L.B., Ahrens, A.H. (1987). Depression and pessimism for the future: Biased use of statistically relevant information in predictions for self versus others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 366– 378 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Borkenau, P., Mauer, N., Riemann, R., Spinath, F.M., Angleitner, A. (2004). Thin slices of behavior as cues of personality and intelligence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 599– 614 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Borkenau, P., Ostendorf, F. (1993). NEO-Fünf-Faktoren-Inventar (NEO-FFI) nach Costa und McCrae . [NEO five-factor inventory according to Costa and McCrae]. Goettingen: Hogrefe First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Carver, C.S., White, T.L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319– 333 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Costa, P.T., McCrae, R.R. (1992). NEO PI-R professional manual . Orlando, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Covey, J.A., Davies, A.D.M. (2004). Are people unrealistically optimistic? It depends on how you ask them. British Journal of Health Psychology, 9, 39– 49 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cronbach, L.J. (1955). Processes affecting scores on “understanding of others” and “assumed similarity.”. Psychological Bulletin, 52, 177– 193 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Elliot, A.J., Thrash, T.M. (2002). Approach-avoidance motivation in personality: Approach and avoidance temperaments and goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 804– 818 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eysenck, M.W. (1997). Anxiety and cognition: A unified theory . Hove, UK: Psychology Press First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gosling, S.D., Rentfrow, P.J., Swann, W.B. (2003). A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 504– 528 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Headey, B., Wearing, A. (1989). Personality, life events, and subjective well-being: Toward a dynamic equilibrium model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 731– 739 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Helweg-Larsen, M., Shepperd, J.A. (2001). Do moderators of the optimistic bias affect personal or target risk estimates? A review of the literature. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 74– 95 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Horn, J.L. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrica, 30, 179– 185 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hunter, J.E., Schmidt, F.L. (1990). Methods of meta analysis: Correcting errors and bias in research findings . Newbury Park, CA: Sage First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kenny, D.A. (1994). Interpersonal perception: A social relations analysis . New York: Guilford First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Klar, Y., Giladi, E.E. (1997). No one in my group can be below the group's average: A robust positivity bias in favor of anonymous peers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 885– 901 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Krohne, H.W., Egloff, B., Kohlmann, C.W., Tausch, A. (1996). Untersuchungen mit einer deutschen Version der “Positive and Negative Affect Schedule” (PANAS) [Studies on a German version of the “Positive and Negative Affect Schedule” (PANAS)]. Diagnostica, 42, 139– 156 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kruger, J., Burrus, J. (2004). Egocentrism and focalism in unrealistic optimism (and pessimism). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 332– 340 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kwan, V.S.Y., John, O.P., Kenny, D.A., Bond, M.H., Robins, R.W. (2004). Reconceptualizing individual differences in self-enhancement bias: An interpersonal approach. Psychological Review, 111, 94– 110 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McCrae, R.R., Costa, P.T. (1987). Validation of the five-factor model of personality across instruments and observers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 81– 90 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McCrae, R.R., Zonderman, A.B., Costa, P.T., Bond, M.H., Paunonen, S.V. (1996). Evaluating replicability of factors in the revised NEO Personality Inventory: Confirmatory factor analysis versus Procrustes rotation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 552– 566 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Muck, P.M., Hell, B., Gosling, S.D. (submitted). Construct validation of a short five-factor model instrument: A self-peer study on the German adaptation of the Ten Item Personality Inventory (TIPI-G) . Manuscript under review First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ostendorf, F., Angleitner, A. (2004). NEO-Persönlichkeitsinventar nach Costa und McCrae - Revidierte Form [NEO Personality Inventory according to Costa and McCrae - Revised Version] . Göttingen: Hogrefe First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pietromonaco, P.R., Markus, H. (1985). The nature of negative thoughts in depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 799– 807 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Price, P.C., Smith, A.R., Lench, H.C. (2006). The effect of target group size on risk judgments and comparative optimism: The more, the riskier. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 382– 398 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Suh, E., Diener, E., Fujita, F. (1996). Events and subjective well-being: Only recent events matter. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1091– 1102 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Watson, D., Clark, L.A., Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063– 1070 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Weinstein, N.D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 806– 820 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zelenski, J.M., Larsen, R.J. (2002). Predicting the future: How affect-related personality traits influence likelihood judgments for future events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1000– 1010 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar