Skip to main content
Log in

Do the Seconds Turn Into Hours? Relationships between Sustained Pupil Dilation in Response to Emotional Information and Self-Reported Rumination

  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined relationships between self-reported rumination and sustained pupil dilation, an index of cognitive and emotional processing, in response to emotional information in depressed and never-depressed individuals. Pupil dilation was measured during tasks that required alternating emotional and nonemotional processing. Depressed individuals displayed more sustained pupil dilation in response to stimuli on emotional processing tasks than nondepressed individuals. Such sustained pupil dilation among depressed individuals was particularly apparent in response to negative and personally relevant emotional information. Multiple self-report measures of rumination were moderately correlated with sustained pupil dilation to negative personally relevant information. Results are consistent with the idea that sustained emotional processing of briefly presented stimuli may be associated with the propensity for depressive rumination.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author

    Google Scholar 

  • Beatty, J. (1982a). Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 276–292.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beatty, J. (1982b). Phasic not tonic pupillary responses vary with auditory vigilance performance. Psychophysiology, 19, 167–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New York: Hoeber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Steer, R., & Garbin, M. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 8, 77–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1997). Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW): Technical manual and affective ratings. Gainsville, FL: University of Florida, The Center for Research in Psychophysiology.

    Google Scholar 

  • First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L, Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1997). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Personality Disorders (SCID-II). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fritz, H. L. (1999). Rumination and adjustment to a first coronary event. Psychosomatic Medicine, 61(1), 105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Granholm, E., Asarnow, R. F., Sarkin, A. J., & Dykes, K. L. (1996). Pupillary responses index cognitive resource limitations. Psychophysiology, 33, 457–461.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, D., & Buchwald, J. (1991). Significance testing of difference potentials. Psychophysiology, 28, 240–244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartwell, J. W. (1995). EEGSYS user's guide (version 5.7). Baltimore, MD: Friends Medical Science Research Center

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollon, S. D., & Kendall, P. C. (1980). Cognitive self-statements in depression: Development of an Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 4, 383–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz, M. J., Wilner, N., & Alvarez, W. (1979). Impact of Event Scale: A measure of subjective stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 41, 209–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingram, R. E. (1984). Toward an information processing analysis of depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8, 443–478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingram, R. E. (1990). Self-focused attention in clinical disorders: Review and a conceptual model, Psychological Bulletin, 107, 156–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingram, R. E., Miranda, J., & Segal, Z. V. (1998). Cognitive vulnerability to depression. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janisse, M. P. (1974). Pupil size, affect, and exposure frequency. Social Behavior and Personality, 2, 125–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Just, N., & Alloy, L. B. (1997). The response styles theory of depression: Tests and an extension of the theory. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 221–229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Beatty, J. (1966). Pupil diameter and load on memory. Science, 154, 1583–1585.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luminet, O., Rime, B., & Wagner, H. (2002). Intrusive thoughts in the laboratory and their long-lasting consequences. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. M. (1991). Cognitive-experimental approaches to the emotional disorders. In P. R. Martin (Ed.), Handbook of behavior therapy and psychological science: An integrative approach (Vol. 164, pp. 116–150). New York: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (1989). Toward a motivational and structural theory of ruminative thought. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 306–326). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, G., & Southall, A. (1991). Depression and the processing of emotional stimuli: A study of semantic priming, Cognitive Therapy and Research, 15 (4), 283–302.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569–582.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Morrow, J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1993). Response styles and the duration of episodes of depressed mood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 20–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papageorgiou, C., & Wells, A. (1999). Process and meta-cognitive dimensions of depressive and anxious thoughts and relationships with emotional intensity. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 6, 152–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Philippot, P., & Rime, B. (1998). Social and cognitive processing in emotion: A heuristic for psychopathology. In W. F. Flack & J. D. Laird (Eds.), Emotions in psychopathology (pp. 114–130). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roger, D., & Najarian, B. (1989). The construction and validation of a new scale for measuring emotion control. Personality and Individual Differences, 10(8), 845–853.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegle, G. J. (1994). The Balanced Affective Word List Creation Program. Retrieved from http://www. sci.sdsu.edu/CAL/wordlist/

  • Siegle, G. J. (1999). A neural network model of attention biases in depression. In J. A. Reggia, E. Ruppin, & D. L. Glanzman (Eds.), Disorders of brain, behavior, and cognition: The neurocomputational perspective (pp. 415–441). New Jersey: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegle, G. J. (2001). Convergence and divergence in self-report measures of rumination and its relationship to depressive symptomatology. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Siegle, G. J., Granholm, E., Ingram, R. E., & Matt, G. E. (2001). Pupillary response and reaction time measures of sustained processing of negative information in depression. Biological Psychiatry, 49, 624–636.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegle, G. J., Steinhauer, S. R., Thase, M. E., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2002). Can't shake that feeling: fMRI assessment of sustained amygdala activity in response to emotional information in depressed individuals. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 693–707.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegle, G. J., & Thayer, J. T. (in press). Physiological aspects of depressive rumination. In C. Papageorgiou & A. Wells (Eds.), Depressive rumination: Nature, theory and treatment. New York: Wiley.

  • Steinhauer, S. R., Condray, R., & Kasparek, A. (2000). Cognitive modulation of midbrain function: Task-induced reduction of the pupillary light reflex. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 39, 21–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steinhauer, S. R., & Hakerem, G. (1992). The pupillary response in cognitive psychophysiology and schizophrenia. In D. Friedman & G. E. Bruder (Eds.), Psychophysiology and experimental psychopathology: A tribute to Samuel Sutton (pp. 182–204). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.

  • Sternberg, S. (1969). Memory scanning: Mental processes revealed by reaction-time experiments. American Scientist, 57, 421–457.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szabadi, E., & Bradshaw, C. M. (1996). Autonomic pharmacology of a 2-adrenoreceptors. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 10(S3), 6–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D. (1988). Cognitive vulnerability to persistent depression. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 247–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 247–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, A. (2000). Emotional Disorders and Metacognition: Innovative Cognitive Therapy. New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, A., & Davies, M. I. (1994). The Thought Control Questionnaire: A measure of individual differences in the control of unwanted thoughts. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 871–878.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. M. G., & Oaksford, M. (1992). Cognitive science, anxiety, and depression: From experiments to connectionism. In D. J. Stein & J. E. Young (Eds.), Cognitive science and the clinical disorders (pp. 129–150). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Greg J. Siegle.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Siegle, G.J., Steinhauer, S.R., Carter, C.S. et al. Do the Seconds Turn Into Hours? Relationships between Sustained Pupil Dilation in Response to Emotional Information and Self-Reported Rumination. Cognitive Therapy and Research 27, 365–382 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023974602357

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023974602357

Navigation