Skip to main content
Log in

Reading skills in three orthographies: The case of trilingual Arabic–Hebrew–English-speaking Arab children

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study is an investigation into theinterconnections among three languages, Arabic,Hebrew and English, each of which has adifferent orthography. Hebrew and Arabic havemore in common with each other than withEnglish; they are considered ``shallow''orthographies if vowelized and ``deep''orthographies if unvowelized. The reading,language, and working memory skills of 70trilingual Israeli-Arab students, aged 14–15,were assessed. Arabic was their maininstructional language in school, and Hebrewand English were studied as required subjects.All these adolescents studied Hebrew from thethird grade and English from the fourth grade.They were administered word and pseudowordreading, language, orthographic, and workingmemory tests in Arabic, Hebrew, and English. Inspite of differences among these threelanguages, the majority of the children showedadequate proficiency in three languages. Asignificant relationship was found between theacquisition of word and pseudoword readingskills, working memory, and syntactic awarenessskills within and across the three languages.Trilingualism of this nature seems not to havenegative consequences (and may even havepositive consequences) for the development oforal language and reading skills in the threelanguages in spite of their differentorthographies.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abu-Rabia, S. & Siegel, L.S. (2002). Reading, writing, orthographic, phonological, syntactic and memory skills of bilingual Arabic-English speaking Arab children in Canada. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 31, 661-678.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abu-Rabia, S. & Siegel, L.S. (1995). Different orthographies, different context effects: The effects of Arabic sentence context in skilled and poor readers. Reading Psychology: An International Quarterly 16, 1-19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ball, B.W. (1993). Phonological awareness.What's important to whom? Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 5, 141-159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beaumont, J.G. (1982). Studies with verbal stimuli. In J.G. Beaumont (ed.), Divided visual field studies of cerebral organization (pp. 58-86). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowey, J.A. & Francis, J. (1991). Phonological analysis as a function of age and exposure to reading instruction. Applied Psycholinguistics 12, 91-121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L. & Bryant, P. (1978). Difficulties in auditory organization as a possible cause of reading backwardness. Nature (London) 271, 746-747.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradshaw, J.L. & Nettleton, N.C. (1983). Human cerebral asymmetry. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brent-Palmer, C. (1979). A sociolinguistic assessment of the notion of immigrant semilingualism from a social conflict perspective. Working Papers on Bilingualism 17, 135-180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, P.E. (1981). Field testing the theory of double semilingualism. Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In G. Underwood (ed.), Strategies of informatjon processing (pp. 151-216). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, J. (1981). The role of primary language development in promoting educational success for language minority students. In California State Department of Education, Schooling and language minority students: A theoretical framework. Evaluating, Dissemination and Assessment Center, California State University, Los Angeles.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, J. (1989). Empowerment of minority children. Sacramento, CA: California Association for Bilingual Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, J., Swain, M.K., Nakajima, K., Handscombe, J., Green, D. & Trans, C. (1984). Linguistic interdependence among Japanese and Vietnamese immigrant students. In C. Rivera (ed.), Communicative competence approaches to language proficiency assessment: Research and application (pp. 60-81). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.

    Google Scholar 

  • da Fontoura, H. & Siegel, L.S. (1995). Reading, syntactic and working memory skills of bilingual Portuguese-Canadian children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 7, 139-153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durgunoglu, A.Y., Nagy, W.E. & Hacin-Bhatt, B.J. (1993). Cross-language transfer of phonological awareness. Journal of Educational Psychology 85, 453-465.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K.I. & Chambers, S. (1973). Lexical access and naming time. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 12, 627-635.

    Google Scholar 

  • Genesee, F. (1979). Acquisition of reading skills in immersion programs. Foreign Language Annals 12, 71-77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E. & Siegel, L.S. (2000). Orthographic and cognitive factors in the concurrent development of basic reading skill in two languages. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 12, 1-30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jastak, S. & Wilkinson, G.S. (1984). TheWide Range Achievement Test - revised. Wilmington, DE: Jastak Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lambert, W.E. & Tucker, R.G. (1972). Bilingual education of children: The St. Lambert experiment. Rowley, Massachusetts: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mace-Matluck, B.J. & Hoover, W.A. (1980, May). Bilingual reading: Effects of learner characteristics and type of instruction on reading achievement. Paper presented at the 25th annual convention of the International Reading Association, St. Louis, MO.

  • Mace-Matluck, B.J., Hoover, W.A. & Calfee, R.C. (1984). Teaching reading to bilingual children: Final report. Austin, Texas: Southwest Educational Development Laboratory.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, D.C., Schravanevelt, R.W. & Ruddy, M.G. (1974). Loci of contextual effects on word recognition. In P.M.A. Rabbitt & S. Dornic (eds.), Attention and performance, Vol. 5 (pp. 98-118). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R., Kliegel, R., Davidson, B.J. & Foltz, C. (1985). Individual and developmental differences in reading disability. In T.G. Wailer (ed.), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice (pp. 1-64). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramirez, J.D., Yuen, S.D. & Ramey, D.R. (1991). Executive summary, Final report: Longitudinal study of structured English immersion strategy, early exit and late exit transitional bilingual education programs for language minority children. San Mateo, CA: Aguirre International.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimron J. (1993). The role of vowels in reading: A review of studies of English and Hebrew. Psychological Bulletin 114, 52-67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimron, J. & Navon, D. (1981). The distribution of information within letters. Perception and Psychophysics 30, 483-491.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimron, J. & Sivan, T. (1994). Reading proficiency and orthography: Evidence from Hebrew and English. Language Learning 44, 5-27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L.S. (1985). Psycholinguistic aspects of reading disabilities. In L.S. Siegel & F.J. Morrison (eds.), Cognitive development in atypical children. New York: Springer Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L.S. & Ryan, E.B. (1988a). The development of working memory in normally achieving and subtypes of disabled children. Child Development 60, 973-980.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L.S. & Ryan, E.B. (1988b). Development of grammatical sensitivity, phonological, and short-form memory skills in normally achieving and learning disabled children. Developmental Psychology 24, 28-37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L.S. & Ryan, E.B. (1989). Subtypes of developmental dyslexia: The influence of definitional variables. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 1, 257-278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L.S., Share, D. & Geva, E. (1995). Evidence for superior orthographic skills in dyslexics. Psychological Science 6, 250-254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skjelfjord, V.J. (1987). Phonemic segmentation. An important subskill in learning to read. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research 31, 41-58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skutnabb-Kangas, T. & Toukomaa, P. (1976). Teaching migrant children's mother tongue and learning the language of the host country in the context of the sociocultural situation of the migrant family. Helsinki: The Finnish National Commission for UNESCO.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M.J. (1980). The development of grapheme-phoneme correspondence in normal and dyslexic readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 29, 294-305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1982). Individual differences in the cognitive processes of reading: Word decoding. Journal of Learning Disabilities 15, 485-493.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (l988a). Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and garden variety poor reader: The phonological-core variance-difference model. Journal of Learning Disabilities 21, 590-604, 612.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.B. (l988b). The right and wrong places to look for the cognitive locus of reading disability. Annals of Dyslexia 38, 154-177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Troike, R.C. (1984). SCALP: Social and cultural aspects of language proficiency. In C. Rivera (ed.), Language proficiency and academic achievement (pp. 44-54). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W.E., Herriman, M.L. & Nesdale, A.R. (1988). Metalinguistic abilities and beginning reading. Reading Research Quarterly 33, 134-158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verhoeven, L. (1994). Transfer in bilingual development: The linguistic interdependence hypothesis revisited. Language Learning 44, 381-415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, D., Spratt, J.E. & Ezzaki, A. (1989). Does learning to read in a second language always put the child at a disadvantage? Counterevidence from Morocco. Applied Psycholinguistics 10, 31-48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R.K. (1989). Causal relations between the development of phonological processing abilities and the acquisition of reading skills: A meta-analysis. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 34, 261-279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walters, G.S., Bruck, M. & Siedenberg, M. (1985). Do children use similar processes to read and spell words? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 39, 511-530.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R.W. (1973). Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests. Circle Press, MN: American Guidance Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yopp, H.K. (1988). The validity and reliability of phonemic awareness tests. Reading Research Quarterly 23, 159-177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, A.W., & Ellis, A.W. (1985). Different methods of lexical access for words presented in left and right visual hemifields. Brain and Language 24, 326-368.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Abu-Rabia, S., Siegel, L.S. Reading skills in three orthographies: The case of trilingual Arabic–Hebrew–English-speaking Arab children. Reading and Writing 16, 611–634 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025838029204

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025838029204

Navigation