Skip to main content
Log in

The Development of Implicit Epistemologies During Early and Middle Adulthood

  • Published:
Journal of Adult Development Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined the development of implicit epistemologies of Finnish adults during the last ten years. It was hypothesized that specific adult experiences are stimulating for development. Moreover, a person's exploratory orientation was hypothesized to be related to development. Kitchener and King's model and method of Reflective Judgment provided tools to study adolescents' and adults' epistemologies and their possible development. Participants (N = 59) were given initial (1986–1988) and follow-up (1993–1994) Reflective Judgment interviews. The age range was from 24–50 years in the follow-up groups. The Reflective Judgment mean stage scores clearly increased during the two study periods (M1 = 4.64,M2 = 5.10; F(1, 58) = 32.82, p = .000). Education, in particular education beyond a person's primary profession/occupation, was a strong predictor of development. Also, encountering diversity and exploratory orientation were related to development, but their connections were more complicated. No gender differences were found. The results support the idea that positive changes in thinking and reasoning take place during adulthood.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Alexander, C. N., &Langer, E. J. (Eds.). (1990). Higher stages of human development: Perspectives on adult growth. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armon, C. (1993). Developmental conceptions of good work: A longitudinal study. In J. Demick &P.M. Miller (Eds.), Development in the workplace (pp. 21-37). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armon, C. (1998). Adult moral development, experience, and education. Journal of Moral Education, 27, 345-371.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armon, C., &Dawson, T. L. (1997). Developmental trajectories in moral reasoning across the life span. Journal of Moral Education, 26, 433-453.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baxter Magolda, M. B. (1992). Knowing and reasoning in college: Gender-related patterns in students' intellectual development. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belenky, M. F.,Clinchy, B. M.,Goldberger, N. R., &Tarule, J. M. (1986). Women's ways of knowing. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Botella, L., &Gallifa, J. (1995). A constructivist approach to the development of personal epistemic assumptions and worldviews. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 8, 1-18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyes, M. C. (1987). Implications of emerging doubt for adolescent identity formation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of British Columbia.

  • Boyes, M. C., &Chandler, M. (1992). Cognitive development, epistemic doubt and identity formation in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 21, 277-304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broughton, J. (1978). Development of concepts of self, mind, reality and knowledge. In W. Damon (Ed.), Social cognition: New directions for child development (pp. 75-100). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., &Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 116-131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T.,Petty, R. E.,Feinstein, J. A., &Jarvis, W. B. G. (1996). Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: the life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 2, 197-253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D., &Hansen, J. (1981). Manual fo the SVII-SCII: Strong-Campbell Interest Inventory. Palo Alto: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S., &Smith, C. (1993). On understanding the nature of scientific knowledge. Educational Psychologist, 28, 235-251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chandler, M. (1987). The Othello effect. Essay on the emergence and eclipse of skeptical doubt. Humal Development, 30, 137-159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colby, A., &Kohlberg L. (1987). The measurement of moral judgment. Vols. I & II. Standard issue scoring manual. New York: Cambrigde University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Commons, M. L.,Armon, C.,Richards, F. A.,Schrader D. E.,Farrell, E. W.,Tappan, M. B., &Bauer, N. F. (1989). A multidomain study of adult development. In M. L. Commons,J. Sinnot,F. A. Richards &C. Armon (Eds.), Adult development, Vol. 1, Comparisons and applications of adolescent and adult developmental models (pp. 33-56). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Commons, M. L.,Richards, F. A., &Kuhn, D. (1982). Systematic and metasystematic reasoning: A case for a level of reasoning beyond Piaget's formal operations. Child Development, 53, 1058-1069.

    Google Scholar 

  • Commons, M. L.,Richards, F. A., &Armon, C. (Eds.). (1984). Beyond formal operations. Late Adolescent and Adult Cognitive Development. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Commons, M. L.,Trudeau, E. J.,Stein, S. A.,Richards, F. A., &Krause, S. R. (1998). The existence of developmental stages as shown by the hierarchical complexity of tasks. Developmental Review, 8, 237-278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edmondson, K. M., &Novak, J. D. (1993). The interplay of scientific epistemological views, learning strategies, and attitudes of college students. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 30, 547-559.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: the control and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review, 87, 477-531.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. W., &Silvern, L. (1985). Stages and individual differences in cognitive development. Annual Reviews of Psychology, 36, 613-648.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, O. J.,Hunt, D. E., &Schroder, H. M. (1961). Conceptual systems and personality organization. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holland, J. L. (1973). Making vocational choices: A theory of careers. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jehng, J.-C.,Johnson, S. D., &Anderson, R. C. (1993). Schooling and students' epistemological beliefs about learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 18, 23-25.

    Google Scholar 

  • King, P. M. (1977). The development of reflective judgment and formal operational thinking in adolescents and young adults. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Minnesota.

  • King, P. M., &Kitchener, K. S. (1994). Developing reflective judgment. Understanding and promoting intellectual growth and critical thinking in adolescents and adults. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitchener, K. S. (1978). Intellectual development in late adolescents and young adults: Reflective Judgment and verbal reasoning. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Minnesota.

  • Kitchener, K. S., &King, P. M. (1981). Reflective Judgment: Concepts of justification and their relationship to age and education. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 2, 89-116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitchener, K. S., &King, P. M. (1985). Reflective Judgment scoring rules. Revised edition. Available from the authors.

  • Kitchener, K. S., &King, P. M. (1990). The Reflective Judgment Model: Ten years of research. In M. L. Commons,J. D. Sinnot,F. A. Richards &C. Armon (Eds.), Adult development, Vol. 2, Models and methods in the study of adolescent and adult thought (pp. 63-79). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitchener, K. S.,Lynch, C. L.,Fischer, K. W., &Wood, P. K. (1993). Developmental range of reflective judgment: the effect of contextual support and practice on developmental stage. Developmental Psychology, 29, 893-906.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitchener, K. S., &Wood, P. K. (1987). Development of concepts of justification in German university students. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 10, 171-185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental approach to socialization. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 347-480). Chicago: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L., &Armon, C. (1984). Three types of stage models used in the study of adult development. In M. L. Commons,F. A. Richards, & Armon, C. (Eds.), Beyond formal operations. Late adolescent and adult cognitive development (pp. 383-394). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loevinger, J. (with the assistance of A. Blasi) (1976). Ego development. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loevinger, J., &Wessler, R. (1970). Measuring ego development. Vol. 1, San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J. E.,Silva, D. G.,Newman, J. H., &Thayer, J. F. (1994). An investigation into the structure of epistemological style. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 617-629.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, M. E. (1994). World views, ego development, and epistemological changes from the conventional to the postformal: a longitudinal study. In M. E. Miller &S. R. Cook-Greuter (Eds.), Transcendence and mature thought in adulthood (pp. 147-179). Lantham: Rowman & Littlefield.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perry, W. G., Jr. (1970). Forms of intellectual and ethical development in the college years. A scheme. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pirttilä-Backman, A.-M. (1991). “Knowledge” and “The experts knowledge”: should there be a difference? Working papers 2/ 1991. Department of Social Psychology, University of Helsinki, Finland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pirttilä-Backman, A.-M. (1993). The social psychology of knowledge reassessed. Toward a new delineation of the field with empirical substantiation. Annales Academiae Scientiarum Fennicae. Dissertationes Humanarum Litterarum 68. Helsinki, Finland: Academia Scientiarum Fennica.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards, F. A., &Commons, M. L. (1984). Systematic, metasystematic, and cross-paradigmatic reasoning: a case for stages of reasoning beyond formal operations. In M. L. Commons,F. A. Richards, &C. Armon (Eds.), Beyond formal operations. Late adolescent and adult cognitive development (pp. 92-119). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and adolescent self-image. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 80 (1).

  • Schaie, K. W. (1996). Intellectual development in adulthood. The Seattle longitudinal study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schommer, M. (1993). Epistemological development and academic performance among secondary students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 406-411.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinsley, H. E. A., &Weiss, D. J. (1975). Interrater reliability and agreement of subjective judgments. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 22, 358-376.

    Google Scholar 

  • United Nations Development Programme (UNDP). (1995). HumanDevelopment Report 1995. Recognizing women's contribution. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valecha, G. K. (1972). Construct validation of internal-external locus of control as measured by an abbreviated 11-item IE scale. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. The Ohio State University.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Pirttilä-Backman, AM., Kajanne, A. The Development of Implicit Epistemologies During Early and Middle Adulthood. Journal of Adult Development 8, 81–97 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026441801408

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026441801408

Navigation