Skip to main content
Log in

Evidence for an Error Monitoring Deficit in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We studied error monitoring in ADHD and control children in a task requiring inhibition of a motor response. The extent of slowing following successful (stopped) and failed (nonstopped) inhibition was compared across groups. We also measured the time required to inhibit a response (stop signal reaction time, SSRT). Compared to controls, ADHD participants slowed less following nonstopped responses. Slowing did not vary with comorbid reading, oppositional, conduct or anxiety disorder, sex or ADHD subtype. Slowing after nonstopped responses was marginally, although significantly correlated with total ADHD symptoms and with age. ADHD participants had significantly longer SSRT than controls, but SSRT was not significantly correlated with slowing. The apparent deficit in error monitoring in ADHD and its independence from the inhibition deficit observed in ADHD has implications for executive control models of ADHD, performance problems associated with the disorder and for component theories of executive control.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders—IV. Washington, DC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 65-94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bedard, A. C., Nichols, S., Barbosa, J. A., Schachar, R., Logan, G. D., & Tannock, R. (2002). The development of selective inhibitory control across the life span. Developmental Neuropsychology, 21(1), 93-111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, P. S., Scheffers, M. K., & Coles, M. G. (1995). "Where did I go wrong?" A psychophysiological analysis of error detection. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 21(6), 1312-1322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624-652.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyle, M. H., Offord, D. R., Racine, Y., Fleming, J. E., Szatmari, P., & Sanford, M. (1993). Evaluation of the revised Ontario Child Health Study scales. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34(2), 189-213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Gray, J. R., Molfese, D. L., & Snyder, A. (2001). Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: Effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. Cerebral Cortex, 11(9), 825-836.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chevrier, A., Noseworthy, M., & Schachar, R. (2003). Neural activity associated with failed inhibition: An event related fMRI study of performance monitoring [Abstract]. Rotman Institute Research Conference, Toronto, March, 2003.

  • Crosbie, J., & Schachar, R. (2001). Deficient inhibition as a marker for familial ADHD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(11), 1884-1890.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Posner, M. I., & Tucker, D. M. (1994). Localization of a neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychological Science, 5(5), 303-305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dikman, Z. V., & Allen, J. J. (2000). Error monitoring during reward and avoidance learning in high-and low-socialized individuals. Psychophysiology, 37(1), 43-54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, J. (1995). Attention, intelligence, and the frontal lobes. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 721-733). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eslinger, P. J., & Damasio, A. R. (1985). Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation: Patient EVR. Neurology, 35(12), 1731-1741.

    Google Scholar 

  • Falkenstein, M., Hohnsbein, J., Hoormann, J., & Blanke, L. (1991). Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II: Error processing in choice reaction tasks. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 78(6), 447-455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Falkenstein, M., Hoormann, J., Christ, S., & Hohnsbein, J. (2000). ERP components on reaction errors and their functional significance: A tutorial. Biological Psychology, 51(2–3), 87-107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Shaywitz, S. E., & Lyon, G. R. (1998). Intelligent testing and the discrepancy model for children with learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 13, 186-203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garavan, H., Ross, T. J., Murphy, K., Roche, R. A. P., & Stein, E. A. (2002). Dissociable executive functions in the dynamic control of behavior: Inhibition, error processing, and correction. Neuroimage, 17, 1820-1829.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gehring, W. J., Coles, M. G. H., Meyers, D. E., & Donchin, E. (1990). The error-related negativity: An event-related potential accompanying errors. Psychophysiology, 27, S34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gehring, W. J., & Fencsik, D. E. (2001). Functions of the medial frontal cortex in the processing of conflict and errors. Journal of Neuroscience, 21(23), 9430-9437.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gehring, W. J., Goss, B., Coles, M. G. H., Meyers, D. E., & Donchin, E. (1993). A neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychological Science, 4, 385-390.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gehring, W. J., Himle, J., & Nisenson, L. G. (2000). Action-monitoring dysfunction in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Psychological Science, 11(1), 1-6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gehring, W. J., & Knight, R. T. (2000). Prefrontal-cingulate interactions in action monitoring. Nature Neuroscience, 3(5), 516-520.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gemba, H., Sasaki, K., & Brooks, V. B. (1986). "Error" potentials in limbic cortex (anterior cingulate area 24) of monkeys during motor learning. Neuroscience Letters, 70(2), 223-227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1995). Architecture of the prefrontal cortex and the central executive. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 769, 71-83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hajcak, G., & Simons, R. F. (2002). Error-related brain activity in obsessive–compulsive undergraduates. Psychiatry Research, 110(1), 63-72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holroyd, C. B., & Coles, M. G. (2002). The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review, 109(4), 679-709.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ickowicz, A., Sugarman, R., Millette, C., Schachar, R., Chen, S., & Orechovsky, V. (2004). Parent Interview for Child Symptoms: Reliability, predictive and concurrent validity. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Kindlon, D., Mezzacappa, E., & Earls, F. (1995). Psychometric properties of impulsivity measures: Temporal stability, validity and factor structure. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36(4), 645-661.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kleiter, G. D., & Schwarzenbacher, K. (1989). Beyond the answer: Post-error processes. Cognition, 32(3), 255-277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kopp, B., & Rist, F. (1999). An event-related brain potential substrate of disturbed response monitoring in paranoid schizophrenic patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108(2), 337-346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, A. F., Humphrey, D. G., Larish, J. F., Logan, G. D., & Strayer, D. L. (1994). Aging and inhibition: Beyond a unitary view of inhibition processing in attention. Psychology and Aging, 9, 491-512.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krusch, D. A., Klorman, R., Brumaghim, J. T., Fitzpatrick, P. A., Borgstedt, A. D., & Strauss, J. (1996). Methylphenidate slows reactions of children with attention deficit disorder during and after an error. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 24(5), 633-650.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leth-Steensen, C., Elbaz, Z. K., & Douglas, V. I. (2000). Mean response times, variability, and skew in the responding of ADHD children: A response time distributional approach. Acta Psychologica (Amst), 104(2), 167-190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1985). On the ability to inhibit simple thought and action. II: Stop signal studies of repetition priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11(4), 675-691.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A users' guide to the stop signal paradigm. In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibirtory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 189-239). San Diego: Academic press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D., & Cowan, W. B. (1984). On the aiblity to inhibit thought and action: A theory of an act of control. Psychological Review, 91(3), 295-327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D., Schachar, R. J., & Tannock, R. T. (1997). Impulsivity and inhibitory control. Psychological Science, 8, 60-64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luna, B., & Sweeney, J. A. (2001). Studies of brain and cognitive maturation through childhood and adolescence: A strategy for testing neurodevelopmental hypotheses. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 27(3), 443-455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luna, B., Thulborn, K. R., Munoz, D. P., Merriam, E. P., Garver, K. E., Minshew, N. J., et al. (2001). Maturation of widely distributed brain function subserves cognitive development. Neuroimage, 13(5), 786-793.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luu, P., Flaisch, T., & Tucker, D. M. (2000). Medial frontal cortex in action monitoring. Journal of Neuroscience, 20(1), 464-469.

    Google Scholar 

  • March, J. S., Parker, J. D., Sullivan, K., Stallings, P., & Connors, C. K. (1997). The Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC): Factor structure, reliability, and validity. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(4), 554-565.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "Frontal Lobe" tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49-100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niki, H., & Watanabe, M. (1979). Prefrontal and cingulate unit activity during timing behavior in the monkey. Brain Research, 171(2), 213-224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation Vol. 4, (pp. 1-18). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(1), 51-87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pujol, J., Lopez, A., Deus, J., Cardoner, N., Vallejo, J., Capdevila, A., et al. (2002). Anatomical variability of the anterior cingulate gyrus and basic dimensions of human personality. Neuroimage, 15(4), 847-855.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rabbitt, P. M. (1966a). Error correction time without external error signals. Nature, 212(60), 438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rabbitt, P. M. (1966b). Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(2), 264-272.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rabbitt, P. M. (1968). Three kinds of error-signalling responses in a serial choice task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20(2), 179-188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rabbitt, P. M., & Roger, B. (1977). What does a man do after he makes an error? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 727-743.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiger, M., & Gauggel, S. (1999). Inhibitory after-effects in the stop signal paradigm. British Journal of Psychology, 90, 509-518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rylander, G. (1947). Personality analysis before and after frontal lobotomy. In C. D. Aring, J. F. Fulton, & B. S. Wortis (Ed.), Research publications association for research in nervous and mental disease: The frontal lobes (pp. 691-705). Baltimore, MD: Willimas & Wilkins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schachar, R., Ickowicz, A., Crosbie, J., Reiz, J., Miceli, P., Harsanyi, Z., et al. (2001). Evaluation of controlled-release methylphenidate in the treatment of ADHD. Paper presented at the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Honolulu, Hawaii.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schachar, R., Mota, V. L., Logan, G. D., Tannock, R., & Klim, P. (2000). Confirmation of an inhibitory control deficit in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28(3), 227-235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheffers, M. K., & Coles, M. G. (2000). Performance monitoring in a confusing world: Error-related brain activity, judgments of response accuracy, and types of errors. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 26(1), 141-151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheffers, M. K., Coles, M. G., Bernstein, P., Gehring, W. J., & Donchin, E. (1996). Event-related brain potentials and error-related processing: An analysis of incorrect responses to go and no-go stimuli. Psychophysiology, 33(1), 42-53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Semel, E., Wiig, E. I., & Secord, W. A. (1995). CELF-3 clinical evaluation of language fundamentals. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sergeant, J. (2000). The cognitive-energetic model: An empirical approach to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuroscience Biobehavioral Review, 24(1), 7-12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sergeant, J. A., & van der Meere, J. (1988). What happens after a hyperactive child commits an error? Psychiatry Research, 24(2), 157-164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T., & Burgess, P. (1996). The domain of supervisory processes and temporal organization of behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Biological Science, 351(1346), 1405-1411; discussion 1411–1402.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T., Marzocchi, G. M., Coser, S., Del Savio, M., Meuter, R. F., & Rumiati, R. I. (2002). Executive function profile of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Neuropsychology, 21(1), 43-71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M., Holahan, J. M., & Shaywitz, S. E. (1992). Discrepancy compared to low achievement definitions of reading disability: Results from the Connecticut Longitudinal Study. Journal of Learning Disorders, 25, 635-648.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2002). Psychological heterogeneity in AD/HD—A dual pathway model of behaviour and cognition. Behavioral Brain Research, 130(1–2), 29-36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Williams, E., Hall, M., & Saxton, T. (1996). Hyperactivity and delay aversion III: The effect on cognitive style of imposing delay after errors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(2), 189-194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stuphorn, V., Taylor, T. L., & Schall, J. D. (2000). Performance monitoring by the supplementary eye field. Nature, 408(6814), 857-860.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tannock, R., Hum, M., Masellis, M., Humphries, T., & Schachar, R. (2002). Teacher Telephone Interview for children's academic performance, attention, behavior, and learning: DSM-IV Version (TTI-IV) Unpublished manuscript, Toronto, Canada: The Hospital for Sick Children.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tow, P. M., & Whitty, C. W. (1953). Personality changes after operations on the cingulate gyrus in man. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 16, 186-193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Boxtel, G. J., Van der Molen, M. W., Jennings, J. R., & Brunia, C. H. (2001). A psychophysiological analysis of inhibitory motor control in the stop-signal paradigm. Biological Psychology, 58(3), 229-262.

    Google Scholar 

  • van der Molen, M.W. (2000). Developmental changes in inhibitory processing: Evidence from psychophysiological measures. Biological Psychology, 54(1–3), 207-239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1991a). Wechsler Individual Achievement Test. San Antionio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1991b). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, G. S. (1993). Wide Range Achievement Test-Revision 3. Wilmington, DE: Jastak Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, B. R., Ponesse, J. S., Schachar, R. J., Logan, G. D., & Tannock, R. (1999). Development of inhibitory control across the life span. Developmental Psychology, 35(1), 205-213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock Reading Mastery Test-Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Schachar, R.J., Chen, S., Logan, G.D. et al. Evidence for an Error Monitoring Deficit in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol 32, 285–293 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JACP.0000026142.11217.f2

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JACP.0000026142.11217.f2

Navigation