Skip to main content
Log in

Training of poor readers' phonological decoding skills: Evidence for syllable-bound processing

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A flash card computer program designed to increase decoding speed in reading was evaluated with a sample of 55 eight- to twelve-year-old Dutch poor readers by using a pretest-training-posttest control group design. The reading level of the poor readers was comparable to the reading level of normal readers in grade two. After 8 weeks of practicing (twice a week) for 30 minutes per session, children in the training group showed substantial improvements in decoding speed not only on pseudowords directly practiced during the training, but also in untrained pseudowords and in existing words. The no-training control group did not improve in decoding speed from pre- to posttest. The results, furthermore, indicate that syllable-bound processes play a role in phonological decoding in Dutch, and that the flash card method used in this study stimulates poor readers to decode letter strings into syllabic units, which leads to an increase in decoding speed. The role of the syllable in phonological decoding and the potential of a specific flash card technique as a means to improve decoding skills of poor readers are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, M.J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Badian, N.A. (1994). Do dyslexic and other poor readers differ in reading-related cognitive skills?, Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 6: 45–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beech, J.R. & Harding, L.M. (1984). Phonemic processing and the poor reader from a developmental lag viewpoint, Reading Research Quarterly 19: 357–366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butler, B.E., Jared, D. & Hains, S. (1984). Reading skill and the use of orthographic knowledge by mature readers, Psychological Research 46: 337–353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In G. Underwood (ed.), Strategies of Information Processing (pp. 151–216). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P. & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches, Psychological Review 100: 589–608.

    Google Scholar 

  • Das-Smaal, E.A., Klapwijk, M.J.G. & van der Leij, A. (1996). Training of perceptual unit processing in children with a reading disability, Cognition and Instruction 14: 221–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L.C. & Wilce, L.S. (1983). Development of word identification speed in skilled and less skilled beginning readers, Journal of Educational Psychology 75: 3–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, C.W., Pollack, M.D. & Montague, W.E. (1970). Implicit speech: Mechanism in perceptual encoding, Journal of Experimental Psychology 84: 502–507.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, L. (1982). Orthography and word recognition in reading. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, L., Coltheart, M. & Woodhouse, D. (1973). Failure to find a syllabic effect in number naming, Memory & Cognition 1: 304–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, K.M., Torgesen, J.K. & Sexton, M. (1987). Using a computer guided practice to increase decoding fluency in learning disabled children: A study using the Hint and Hunt I program, Journal of Learning Disabilities 20: 122–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, L.& Feldman, L.B. (1981). Linguistic coding in word recognition: Comparisons between a deep and a shallow orthography. In A.M. Lesgold & C.A. Perfetti (eds.), Interactive processes in reading (pp. 85–106). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence.

  • Klapp, S.T. (1971). Implicit speech inferred from response latencies in same-different decisions, Journal of Experimental Psychology 91: 262–267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klapp, S.T. (1974). Syllable-dependent pronunciation latencies in number naming: A replication, Journal of Experimental Psychology 102: 1138–1140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klapp, S.T., Anderson, W.G. & Berrian, R.W. (1973). Implicit speech in reading, reconsidered, Journal of Experimental Psychology 100: 368–374.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohnstamm, G.A., Schaerlaekens, A.M., de Vries, A.K., Akkerhuis, G.W. & Froonincksx, M. (1981). Nieuwe streeflijst woordenschat voor 6-jarigen: gebaseerd op onderzoek in Nederland en België [New target list vocabulary for 6-year olds: Based on research in The Netherlands and Belgium]. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D. & Samuels, S.J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading, Cognitive Psychology 6: 293–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lemoine, H.E., Levy, B.A. & Hutchinson, A. (1993). Increasing the naming speed of poor readers: Representations formed across repetitions, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 55: 297–328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lesgold, A. & Resnick, L. (1982). How reading difficulties develop: Perspectives from a longitudinal study. In J. Das, R. Mulcahey & A. Waal (eds.), Theory and research in learning disabilities (pp. 155–187). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W.J.M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovett, M.W., Ransby, M.J., Hardwick, N., Johns, M.S. & Donaldson, S.A. (1989). Can dyslexia be treated? Treatment-specific and generalized treatment effects in dyslexic children's response to remediation, Brain and Language 37: 90–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovett, M.W., Warren-Chaplin, P.M., Ransby, M.J. & Borden, S.L. (1990). Training in word recognition skills of reading disabled children: Treatment and transfer effects, Journal of Educational Psychology 82: 769–780.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F.R. (1985). Acquisition of word identification skills in normal and disabled readers, Journal of Educational Psychology 77: 78–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marmurek, H.H.C. & Rinaldo, R. (1992). The development of letter and syllable effects in categorization, reading aloud, and picture naming, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 53: 277–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason, M. (1978). From print to sound in mature readers as a function of reader ability and two forms of orthographic regularity, Memory & Cognition 6: 568–581.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCormick, C. & Samuels, S.J. (1980). Word recognition by second graders: The unit of perception and interrelationships among accuracy, latency, and comprehension, Journal of Reading Behavior 11: 107–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mewhort, D.J.K. & Beal, A.L. (1977). Mechanisms of word identification, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 3: 629–640.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mewhort, D.J.K. & Campbell, A.J. (1981). Toward a model of skilled reading: An analysis of performance in tachistoscopic tasks. In G.E. Mackinnon & T.G. Waller (eds.), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice (Vol. 3, pp. 39–118). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R.K. & Wise, B.W. (1992). Reading on the computer with orthographic and speech feedback: An overview of the Colorado remediation project, Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 4: 107–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C.A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinzmetal, W., Treiman, R. & Rho, S.H. (1986). How to see a reading unit, Journal of Memory and Language 25: 461–475.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rack, J.P., Snowling, M.J. & Olson, R.K. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: A review, Reading Research Quarterly 27: 29–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reitsma, P. & Verhoeven, L. (1990). Acquisition of reading in Dutch. Dordrecht: Foris Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, S.F. & Beck, I.L. (1987). Theoretical and instructional implications of the assessment of two microcomputer word recognition programs, Reading Research Quarterly 22: 197–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Samuels, S.J., LaBerge, D. & Bremer, C.D. (1978). Units of word recognition: Evidence for developmental changes, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 17: 715–720.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheerer-Neumann, G. (1981). The utilization of intraword structure in poor readers: Experimental evidence and a training program, Psychological Research 43: 155–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smeets, H. & van der Leij, A. (1995). Effecten van een multicomponentieel computergestuurd programma op technisch lezen [Effects of a multi-componential computer-based reading program], Tijdschrift voor Orthopedagogiek 34: 128–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M.J. (1980). The development of grapheme-phoneme correspondence in normal and dyslexic readers, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 29: 294–305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M.J. (1981). Phonemic deficits in developmental dyslexia, Psychological Research 43: 219–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staphorsius, G., Krom, R.S.H. & de Geus, K. (1989). Frequenties van woordvormen en letterposities in jeugdliteratuur [Frequencies of word forms and letter positions in youth literature]. Arnhem: Cito.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, S., Monsell, S., Knoll, R.L. & Wright, C.E. (1978). The latency and duration of rapid movement sequences: Comparisons of speech and typewriting. In G.E. Stelmach (ed.), Information processing in motor control and learning (pp. 118–152). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tousman, S. & Inhoff, A. (1992). Phonology in multisyllabic word recognition, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 21: 525–544.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Bon, W.H.J. (1994). Remediation of reading problems: Effects of training at word and subword level. In E.M.H. Assink (ed.), Literacy acquisition and social context (pp. 150–175). Hemel Hempstead: Harvester Wheatsheaf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Bon, W.H.J., van Kessel, A.E.G. & Kortenhorst, E.P.M. (1987). Beënvloeding van woordherkenningssnelheid door middel van flash cards [Affecting word identification speed by flash cards]. In J. Hamers & A. van der Leij (eds.), Dyslexie 87. Utrecht, The Netherlands (pp. 87–94). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van den Bosch, K. (1991). Poor readers' decoding skills: Effects of training, task, and word characteristics. Doctoral dissertation, University of Nijmegen, The Netherlands.

  • Van den Bosch, K., van Bon, W.H.J. & Schreuder, R. (1995). Poor readers' decoding skills: Effects of training with limited exposure duration, Reading Research Quarterly 30: 110–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van der Leij, A. (1987). Special education in the Netherlands. In C.R. Reynolds & L. Mann (eds.), Encyclopedia of special education (pp. 1094–1095). New York: JohnWiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van der Leij, A. (1994). Effects of computer-assisted instruction on word and pseudoword reading of reading-disabled students. In K.P. van den Bos, L.S. Siegel, D.J. Bakker & D.L. Share (eds.), Current directions in dyslexia research (pp. 251–267). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Heuven, V.J. (1980). Aspects of Dutch orthography and reading. In J.F. Kavanagh & R.L. Venezky (eds.), Orthography, reading, and dyslexia (pp. 57–73). Baltimore: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F.R., Scanlon, D.M. & Spearing, D. (1995). Semantic and phonological coding in poor and normal readers, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 59: 76–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verhoeven, L. (1992). Drie-Minuten-Toets [Three-Minutes-Test]. Arnhem: Cito.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R.K. & Torgesen, J.K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills, Psychological Bulletin 101: 192–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wentink, H.W.M.J. (1997). From graphemes to syllables: The development of phonological decoding skills in poor and normal readers. Doctoral dissertation, University of Nijmegen, The Netherlands. Manuscript in preparation.

  • Yap, R. (1993). Automatic word processing deficits in dyslexia: Qualitative differences and specific remediation. Doctoral dissertation, Free University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

WENTINK, H.W., VAN BON, W.H. & SCHREUDER, R. Training of poor readers' phonological decoding skills: Evidence for syllable-bound processing. Reading and Writing 9, 163–192 (1997). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007921805360

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007921805360

Navigation