Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ndmmz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-01T04:58:29.965Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A multilingual advantage in the components of working memory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 October 2017

KATE COCKCROFT*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, School of Human and Community Development, University of the Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, South Africa
MANDY WIGDOROWITZ
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, School of Human and Community Development, University of the Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, South Africa
LUZANNE LIVERSAGE
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, School of Human and Community Development, University of the Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, South Africa
*
Address for correspondence: Department of Psychology, School of Human and Community Development, University of the Witwatersrand, P.O. WITS, 2050, Johannesburg, South Africa. kate.cockcroft@wits.ac.za

Abstract

This study compared working memory ability in multilingual young adults and their monolingual peers on four components of working memory (verbal and visuospatial storage, verbal and visuospatial processing). The sample comprised 39 monolingual English speakers, and 39 multilinguals, who spoke an African language as their first and third languages, and English as their second language, all with high levels of proficiency. The multilingual young adults came from lower socioeconomic status (SES) backgrounds and possessed smaller English vocabularies than the monolinguals, features which make this group an under-researched population. Both when SES and verbal ability were and were not statistically controlled, there was evidence of a multilingual advantage in all of the working memory components, which was most pronounced in visuospatial processing. These findings support evidence from bilinguals showing cognitive advantages beyond inhibitory control, and suggest that multilingualism may influence the executive control system generally.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

The financial assistance of the National Research Foundation (NRF) towards this research is hereby acknowledged. Opinions expressed, and conclusions arrived at, are those of the author and are not necessarily to be attributed to the NRF.

References

Alloway, T.P. (2007). The Automated Working Memory Assessment. London: Pearson Clinical.Google Scholar
Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., & Pickering, S. J. (2006). Verbal and visuospatial short-term and working memory in children: are they separable? Child Development, 77, 1698–716.Google Scholar
Alloway, T.P., & Passolunghi, M.C.. (2011). The relations between working memory and arithmetical abilities: A comparison between Italian and British children. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 133137.Google Scholar
Antón, E., Duñabeitia, J.A., Estévez, A., Hernández, J.A., Castillo, A., Fuentes, L.J., & Carreiras, M. (2014). Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 17.Google Scholar
Antón, E., García, Y.F., Carreiras, M., & Duñabeitia, J.A. (2016). Does bilingualism shape inhibitory control in the elderly? Journal of Memory and Language, 90, 147160.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A.D. (1986). Working memory. Science, 255, 556559.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A.D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 417423.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A.D. (2012). Working memory, theories, models and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 129.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A.D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. Psychology, Learning and Motivation, 8, 4789.Google Scholar
Baum, S., & Titone, D. (2014). Moving toward a neuroplasticity view of bilingualism, executive control, and aging. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35, 857894.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2008). Lexical access in bilinguals: effects of vocabulary size and executive control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21, 522538Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2009). Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 311.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2017). The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 233262.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Barac, R. (2012). Emerging bilingualism: Dissociating advantages for metalinguistic awareness and executive control. Cognition, 122, 6773.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Barac, R., Blaye, A., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2010). Word mapping and executive functioning in young monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Cognitive Development, 11, 485508.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Craik, F.I.M. (2010). Cognitive and linguistic processing in the bilingual mind. Psychological Science, 19, 1923.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F.I.M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290303.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F.I.M., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 34, 859873.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F.I.M., & Luk, G. (2012). Bilingualism: consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Science, 16, 240250.Google Scholar
Blom, E., Küntay, A.C., Messer, M., Verhagen, J., & Leseman, P. (2014). The benefits of being bilingual: Working memory in bilingual Turkish-Dutch children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 128, 105119.Google Scholar
Bonifacci, P., Giombini, L., Bellocchi, S., & Contento, S. (2011). Speed of processing, anticipation, inhibition and working memory in bilinguals. Developmental Science, 14, 256269.Google Scholar
Brito, N.H., Sebastián-Gallés, N., & Barr, R. (2015). Differences in language exposure and its effects on memory flexibility in monolingual, bilingual, and trilingual infants. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 670682.Google Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J.F. (2009). Monolingual, bilingual, trilingual: infants’ language experience influences the development of a word-learning heuristic. Developmental Science, 12, 815823.Google Scholar
Calvo, N., Ibáñez, A., & Garcia, A.M. (2016). The impact of bilingualism on working memory: A null effect on the whole may not be so on the parts. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 14Google Scholar
Calvo, N., & Bialystok, E. (2014). Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition, 130, 278288.Google Scholar
Carlson, S.M., & Meltzoff, A.N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11, 282298.Google Scholar
Cenoz, J., & Genesee, F. (Eds.) (1998). Beyond bilingualism: Multilingualism and multilingual education. Clevedon, England: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Claassen, N.C.W., Krynauw, A.H., Paterson, H., & Wa ga Mathe, M. (2001). A standardisation of the WAIS-III for English-speaking South Africans. Pretoria: Human Sciences Research Council.Google Scholar
Colzato, L.S., Bajo, M.T., van den Wildenberg, W., Paolieri, D., Nieuwenhuis, S., La Heij, W., & Hommel, B. (2008). How does bilingualism improve executive control? A comparison of active and reactive inhibition mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 34, 302312.Google Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastian-Galles, N. (2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don't. Cognition, 113, 135149.Google Scholar
Cowan, N., & Alloway, T.P. (2008). The development of working memory in childhood. In Courage, M. & Cowan, N. (Eds.), Development of memory in infancy and childhood (2nd Ed., pp. 303342). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
De Bruin, A., Treccani, B., & Della Sala, S. (2014). Cognitive advantage in bilingualism as an example of publication bias? Psychological Science, 26, 19.Google Scholar
De Klerk, V. (2006). Code switching, borrowing and mixing in a corpus of Xhosa English. The International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 9, 597614.Google Scholar
de Pontes Nobre, A., de Carvalho Rodrigues, J., Burges Sbicigo, J., da Rosa Piccolo, L., Zortea, M., Duarte Junior, S., & Fumagalli de Salles, J. (2013). Tasks for assessment of the episodic buffer: A systematic review. Psychology & Neuroscience, 6,331343.Google Scholar
Diamond, J. (2010). The benefits of multilingualism. Science, 330, 332333.Google Scholar
Dong, Y., & Li, P. (2015). The cognitive science of bilingualism. Language and Linguistics Compass, 9/1, 113.Google Scholar
Duñabeitia, J.A.H., Antón, E., Macizo, P., Estévez, A., Fuentes, L.J., & Carreiras, M. (2013). The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: Myth or reality? Experimental Psychology, 61, 234251.Google Scholar
Engel, P. M. J., Santos, F. H., & Gathercole, S. E. (2008). Are working memory measures free of socioeconomic influence? Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 51, 1580–7.Google Scholar
Engel de Abreu, P.M.J. (2011). Working memory in multilingual children: Is there a bilingual effect? Memory, 19, 529537.Google Scholar
Engel de Abreu, P.M.J., Baldassi, M., Puglisi, M.L., & Befi-Lopes, D.M. (2012). Cross-linguistic and cross-cultural effects on verbal working memory and vocabulary. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 56, 630642.Google Scholar
Engel de Abreu, P.M.J., Puglisi, M.L., Cruz-Santos, A., Befi-Lopes, D.M., & Martin, R. (2014). Effects of impoverished environmental conditions on working memory performance. Memory, 22, 232331.Google Scholar
Engle, R.W., Tuholski, S.W., Laughlin, J.E., & Conway, A.R.A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology, General, 128, 309331.Google Scholar
Evans, G., & Schamberg, M. (2009). Childhood poverty, chronic stress, and adult working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 65456549.Google Scholar
Feng, X. (2009). Working memory and bilingualism: An investigation of executive control and processing speed. Unpublished doctoral thesis: York University, Toronto, ON.Google Scholar
Fernandes, M.A., Craik, F.I.M., Bialystok, E., & Kreuger, S. (2007). Effects of bilingualism, aging, and semantic relatedness on meaning under divided attention. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 128141.Google Scholar
Filippi, R., Karamini, T., & Thomas, M.S.C. (2013). Language switching in bilingual production: Empirical data and computational modelling. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 294315.Google Scholar
García-Pentón, L., Fernández García, Y., Costello, B., Duñabeitia, J.A., & Carreiras, M. (2016). The neuroanatomy of bilingualism: how to turn a hazy view into the full picture. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31, 303327.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S.E., Pickering, S.J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Science, 40, 177190.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Knight, C., & Stegmann, Z. (2004). Working memory skills and educational attainment: Evidence from National Curriculum assessments at 7 and 14 years of age. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 116.Google Scholar
Green, D. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 6781.Google Scholar
Green, D. (2011). Language control in different contexts: The behavioural ecology of bilingual speakers. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 103.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F. (1985). The bilingual as a competent but specific speaker-hearer. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 6,467477.Google Scholar
Hartshorne, J.K., & Germine, L.T. (2015). When does cognitive functioning peak? The asynchronous rise and fall of different cognitive abilities across the life span. Psychological Science, 26, 433443.Google Scholar
Hernández, M., Costa, A., Fuentes, L.J., Vivas, A.B., & Sebastian-Galles, N. (2010). The impact of bilingualism on the executive control and orienting networks of attention. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 315325.Google Scholar
Hernández, M., Costa, A., & Humphreys, G.W. (2012). Escaping capture: Bilingualism modulates distraction from working memory. Cognition, 122, 3750.Google Scholar
Hilchey, M.D., & Klein, R.M. (2011). Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin Review, 18, 625658.Google Scholar
Hoshino, N., & Thierry, G. (2011). Language selection in bilingual word production: Electrophysiological evidence for cross-language competition. Brain Research, 1371, 100109.Google Scholar
Humphrey, A.D., & Valian, V.V. (2012). Multilingualism and cognitive control: Simon and flanker task performance in monolingual and multilingual young adults. Paper presented at the 53rd Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society. Minneapolis, MN.Google Scholar
Kane, M.J., Conway, A.R.A., Hambrick, D.Z., & Engle, R.W. (2007). Variation in working memory capacity as variation in executive attention and control. In Conway, A.R.A., Jarrold, C., Kane, M.J., Miyake, A., & Towse, J.N. (Eds.), Variation in working memory (pp. 2148). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kavé, G., Eyal, N., Shorek, A., & Cohen-Mansfield, J. (2008). Multilingualism and cognitive state in the oldest old. Psychology and Aging, 23, 7078.Google Scholar
Kazemeini, T., & Fadardi, J.S. (2015). Executive function: Comparing bilingual and monolingual iranian university students. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, doi: 10.1007/s10936-015-9403-1. Published online by Springer, November 25, 2015.Google Scholar
Klein, R.M. (2015). On the belief that the cognitive exercise associated with the acquisition of a second language enhances extra-linguistic cognitive functions: is type 1 incompetence at work here? Cortex, 73, 340341.Google Scholar
Kline, R.B. (2005). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modelling (2nd Ed.). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Kroll, J.F., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Understanding the Consequences of Bilingualism for Language Processing and Cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 122.Google Scholar
Lawson, G.M., Hook, C.J., Hackman, D.A., & Farah, M.J. (2016). Socioeconomic status and neurocognitive development: Executive function. In Griffin, J.A., Freund, L.S. & McCardle, P. (eds.), Executive function in preschool children: Integrating measurement, neurodevelopment, and translational research. Washington DC: APA Press.Google Scholar
Linck, J.A., Osthus, P., Koeth, J.T., & Bunting, M.F. (2014). Working memory and second language comprehension and production: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin Review, 21, 861883.Google Scholar
Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 605621.Google Scholar
Luk, G., de Sa, E., & Bialystok, E. (2011). Is there a relation between onset age of bilingualism and enhancement of cognitive control? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, 588595.Google Scholar
Luo, L., Craik, F.I.M., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism interacts with domain in a working memory task: Evidence from aging. Psychology and Aging, 28, 2834.Google Scholar
Marian, M., Blumenfeld, H.K., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 50, 940–67.Google Scholar
Martin-Rhee, M.M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11, 8193.Google Scholar
Messer, M.H., Leseman, P.P., Boom, J., & Mayo, A.Y. (2010). Phonotactic probability effect in nonword recall and its relationship with vocabulary in monolingual and bilingual preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 306323.Google Scholar
Mezzacappa, E. (2004). Alerting, orienting and executive attention: Developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Development, 75, 137813386.Google Scholar
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N.P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 814.Google Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N.P., Emerson, M.J, Witzki, A.H., Howerter, A., & Wagner, T.D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contribution to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49100.Google Scholar
Morales, J., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 187202.Google Scholar
Morton, J.B., & Harper, S.N. (2007). What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science, 10, 719726.Google Scholar
Noble, K.G., McCandliss, B.D., & Farah, M.J. (2007). Socioeconomic gradients predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Developmental Science 10, 464480.Google Scholar
Noble, K.G., Norman, M.F., & Farah, M.J. (2005). Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science 8, 7487.Google Scholar
Paap, K.R., & Greenberg, Z.I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66, 232258.Google Scholar
Paap, K.R., Johnson, H.A., & Sawi, O. (2014). Are bilingual advantages dependent upon specific tasks or specific bilingual experiences? Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26, 615639.Google Scholar
Poarch, G.J., & van Hell, J.G. (2012). Executive functions and inhibitory control in multilingual children: Evidence from second-language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113, 535551.Google Scholar
Rinderman, H., Flores-Mendoza, C., & Mansur-Alves, M. (2010). Reciprocal effects between fluid and crystallised intelligence and their dependence on parents’ socioeconomic status and education. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 544548.Google Scholar
Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Lalwani, L.N., & Vélez-Uribe, I. (2015). The effect of language proficiency on executive functions in balanced and unbalanced Spanish-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, doi: 10.1017/S13667289150003091-15. Published online by Cambridge University Press, June 15, 2015. 15 June 2015.Google Scholar
Shook, A., Goldrick, M., Engstler, C., & Marian, V. (2015). Bilinguals show weaker lexical access during spoken sentence comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 44, 789802.Google Scholar
Smithson, L., & Nicoladis, E. (2013). Verbal memory resources predict iconic gesture use among monolinguals and bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 934944.Google Scholar
South African Audience Research Foundation. (2001). Living Standards Measure. Retrieved on 4 March 2013 from: http://www.saarf.co.za/LSM/lsm.aspGoogle Scholar
Spaull, N. (2013). Poverty and privilege: Primary school inequality in South Africa. International Journal of Educational Development, 33, 436447.Google Scholar
Statistics South Africa (2016). Mid-year population estimates 2015. Retrieved on 6 April 2017 from: https://www.statssa.gov.za/publications/P0302/P03022015.pdfGoogle Scholar
Thordardottir, E., Rothenberg, A., Rivard, M.E., & Naves, R. (2006). Bilingual assessment: Can overall proficiency be estimated from separate measurements of two languages? Journal of Multicultural Communication Disorders, 4, 121.Google Scholar
Valian, V. (2015). Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 324.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Third Edition. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wodniecka, Z., Craik, F.I.M., Luo, L., & Bialystok, E. (2010). Does bilingualism help memory? Competing effects of verbal ability and executive control. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13, 575595.Google Scholar
Yang, S., Yang, H., & Lust, B. (2011). Early childhood bilingualism leads to advances in executive attention: Disassociating culture and language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, 412422.Google Scholar