Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-nwzlb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T07:03:49.397Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - What Thoughts Are Made Of

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Gün R. Semin
Affiliation:
Koninklijke Nederlandse Akademie van Wetenschappen, Amsterdam
Eliot R. Smith
Affiliation:
Indiana University, Bloomington
Lera Boroditsky
Affiliation:
Stanford University, CA, USA
Jesse Prinz
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
Get access

Summary

INTRODUCTION

What are thoughts made of? Do we think in pictures? In words? In symbols? What is the currency of human cognition and how do the representations that make up thinking come to be in our minds? In this chapter, we explore the rich sources of input that humans receive from perception and language and how combining information from these two input streams can be used to create the amazing complexity and sophistication of the human knowledge system.

Cognitive science is often seen as emerging from the confluence of two research programs: Chomsky's nativist critique of behaviorist learning theories and the rise of artificial intelligence. Together these two tides lead to a seemingly inevitable pair of conclusions: we think in language-like symbols, and the primitive symbols used in thought are innate. If we think in innate language-like symbols, then obviously we do not think in English, or Russian, or Kuuk Thaayorre. Instead, we think in the universal language of thought – Mentalese” (Fodor, 1975). This conclusion has been explicitly defended by some in cognitive science, but more often it is an unarticulated background assumption. For example, in the literature on concepts and categorization, conceptual representations are often described using structured lists of linguistically labeled features, and researchers rarely suggest that the words used in their theories correspond to mental representations that are radically unlike words.

Type
Chapter
Information
Embodied Grounding
Social, Cognitive, Affective, and Neuroscientific Approaches
, pp. 98 - 116
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baayen, R. H., & Boroditsky, L. (2004). Effects of lexical structure on picture comparison. Edmonton, Canada, Conference: Conceptual Structure, Discourse & language, October 8, 2004.
Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–609.Google ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. W., Simmons, W. K., Barbey, A., & Wilson, C. D. (2003). Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 84–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. (1995). A cognitive developmental approach to morality: Investigating the psychopath. Cognition, 57, 1–29.Google ScholarPubMed
Boroditsky, L. (2000). Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 75(1), 1–28.Google ScholarPubMed
Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought? English and Mandarin speakers' conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43(1), 1–22.Google Scholar
Boroditsky, L., & Gaby, A. (2006). East of Tuesday: Representing time in absolute space. In the Proceedings of the 28th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Vancouver, Canada.Google Scholar
Boroditsky, L., & Ramscar, M. (2002). The roles of body and mind in abstract thought. Psychological Science, 13(2), 185–188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boroditsky, L., & Ramscar, M. (2003). Guilt by association: Gleaning meaning from contextual co-occurrence. Proceedings of the 25th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Boroditsky, L., Schmidt, L. A., & Phillips, W. (2003). Sex, syntax and semantics. In Gentner, D. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (Eds.), Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought (pp. 61–78). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Burgess, C., & Lund, K. (1997). Modeling parsing constraints with high-dimensional context space. Language & Cognitive Processes, 12, 177–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2008). Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casasanto, D., & Lozano, S. (2006). Metaphor in the mind and hands. Proceedings of 28th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Vancouver, Canada. 142–147.Google Scholar
Casasanto, D., & Lozano, S. (in press). Meaning and motor action. Proceedings of 29th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Nashville, TN.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. (1973). Space, time, semantics, and the child. In T. E. Moore, Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Enfield, N. J. (2005). The body as a cognitive artifact in kinship representations. Hand gesture diagrams by speakers of Lao. Current Anthropology, 46, 51–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. (1975). The language of thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. J. (1994). The poetics of mind: Figurative thought, language, and understanding. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Goldstone, R. L., Feng, Y., & Rogosky, B. (2005). Connecting to the world and each other. In Pecher, D. & Zwaan, R. (Eds.), Grounding cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language, and thinking (pp. 292–314). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gomez, R. L., & Gerken, L. A. (1999). Artificial grammar learning by one-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge. Cognition, 70, 109–135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 4, 814–834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobson, R. (1959). On linguistic aspects of translation. In Brower, R. A. (Ed.), On translation (pp. 232–239). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato's problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104, 211–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landauer, T. K., Foltz, P. W., & Laham, D. (1998). An introduction to latent semantic analysis. Discourse Processes, 25, 259–284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, J. (1979). An essay concerning human understanding. P. H. Nidditch (Ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Originally published 1690).
Lund, K., Burgess, C., & Atchley, R. A. (1995). Semantic and associative priming in high-dimensional semantic space. Proceedings of the Seventeenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 660–665). Pittsburgh, PA: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Núñez, R. E., & Sweeter, E. (2006). With the future behind them: Convergent evidence from Aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time, Cognitive Science, 30(3), 401–450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prinz, J. J. (2007). The emotional construction of morals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Redington, M., Chater, N., & Finch, S. (1998). Distributional information: A powerful cue for acquiring syntactic categories, Cognitive Science, 22(4), 425–469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P., Lowery, L., Imada, S., & Haidt, J. (1999). The CAD triad hypothesis: A mapping between three moral emotions (contempt, anger, disgust) and three moral codes (community, autonomy, divinity). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 574–586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saffran, J. R. (2002). Constraints on statistical language learning. Journal of Memory & Language, 47, 172–196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month old infants. Science, 274, 1926–1928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sera, M. D., Berge, C., & Castillo, J. (1994). Grammatical and conceptual forces in the attribution of gender by English and Spanish speakers. Cognitive Development, 9(3), 261–292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepard, R. N., & Cooper, L. A. (1992). Representation of colors in the blind, color–blind, and normally sighted. Psychological Science, 3(2), 97–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shank, R. C., & Abelson, R. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Elbarum.Google Scholar
Traugott, E. C. (1978). On the expression of spatiotemporal relations in language. In Greenberg, J. H. (Ed.), Universals of human language, Vol. 3: Word structure (pp. 369–400). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Valdesolo, P., & DeSteno, D. (2006). Manipulations of emotional context shape moral judgment. Psychological Science, 17(6), 476–477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wheatley, T., & Haidt, J. (2005). Hypnotic disgust makes moral judgments more severe. Psychological Science, 16(10), 780–784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winawer, J., Witthoft, N., Frank, M., Wu, L., Wade, A., & Boroditsky, L. (2007). Russian blues reveal effects of language on color discrimination. PNAS published April 30, 2007, 10.1073/pnas.0701644104.CrossRef

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×