Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-mp689 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T07:39:06.769Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Multilingual Naming Test in Alzheimer's Disease: Clues to the Origin of Naming Impairments

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 January 2013

Iva Ivanova
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
David P. Salmon
Affiliation:
Department of Neurosciences, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
Tamar H. Gollan*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Tamar H. Gollan, 9500 Gilman Drive, 0948, Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093-0948. E-mail: tgollan@ucsd.edu

Abstract

The current study explored the picture naming performance of patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD). First, we evaluated the utility of the Multilingual Naming Test (MINT; Gollan et al., 2011), which was designed to assess naming skills in speakers of multiple languages, for detecting naming impairments in monolingual AD and amnestic mild cognitive impairment (MCI). If the MINT were sensitive to linguistic impairment in AD, using it in clinical practice might have advantages over using tests exclusively designed for English monolinguals. We found that the MINT can be used with both monolinguals and bilinguals: A 32-item subset of the MINT is best for distinguishing monolingual patients from controls, while the full MINT is best for assessing degree of bilingualism and language dominance in bilinguals. We then investigated the cognitive mechanisms underlying naming impairment in AD. To this end, we explored which MINT item characteristics best predicted performance differences between monolingual patients and controls. We found that contextual diversity and imageability, but not word frequency (nor words’ number of senses), contributed unique variance to explaining naming impairments in AD. These findings suggest a semantic component to the naming impairment in AD (modulated by names’ semantic richness and network size). (JINS, 2013, 19, 1–12)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adelman, J.S., Brown, G.D.A., Quesada, J.F. (2006). Contextual diversity, not word frequency, determines word-naming and lexical decision times. Psychological Science, 17, 814823.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allegri, R.F., Mangone, C.A., Fernández-Villavicencio, A., Rymberg, S., Taragano, F., Baumann, D. (1997). Spanish Boston Naming Test norms. Clinical Neuropsychology, 11, 416420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alzheimer's Association. (2004). Hispanics/ Latinos and Alzheimer's disease. Retrieved from http://www.alz.org/national/documents/report–hispanic.pdfGoogle Scholar
Alzheimer's Association. (2008). Alzheimer's disease facts & figures. Alzheimer's and Dementia: The Journal of the Alzheimer's Association, 4, 110–133.Google Scholar
Astell, A.J., Harley, T.A. (1998). Naming problems in dementia: Semantic or lexical? Aphasiology, 12, 357374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balota, D.A., Cortese, M.J., Duchek, J.M., Adams, D., Roediger, H.L. III, McDermott, K.B., Yerys, B.E. (1999). Veridical and false memories in healthy older adults and in dementia of the Alzheimer's type. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 16, 361384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balota, D.A., Duchek, J.M. (1991). Semantic priming effects, lexical repetition effects, and contextual disambiguation effects in healthy aged individuals and individuals with senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. Brain and Language, 40, 181201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barry, C., Morrison, C.M., Ellis, A.W. (1997). Naming the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures: Effects of age of acquisition, frequency, and name agreement. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A, 560585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M., New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 977990.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, M. (1981). The MRC psycholinguistic database. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33A, 497505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Calabria, M., Marne, P., Hernández, M., Juncadella, M., Gascón-Bayarri, J., Reñé, R. (2012). On the parallel deterioration of lexico-semantic processes in the bilinguals’ two languages: Evidence from Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychologia, 50, 740753.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuetos, F., Gonzalez-Nosti, M., Martínez, C. (2005). The picture-naming task in the analysis of cognitive deterioration in Alzheimer's disease. Aphasiology, 19, 545557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuetos, F., Rosci, C., Laiacona, M., Capitani, E. (2008). Different variables predict anomia in different subjects: A longitudinal study of two Alzheimer's patients. Neuropsychologia, 46, 249260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, C.J. (2005). N-Watch: A program for deriving neighborhood size and other psycholinguistic statistics. Behavior Research Methods, 37, 6570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Bot, K. (1992). A bilingual production model: Levelt's speaking model adapted. Applied Linguistics, 13, 124.Google Scholar
Ellis, A.W., Morrison, C.M. (1998). Real age-of-acquisition effects in lexical retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 24, 515523.Google ScholarPubMed
Fellbaum, C. (2005). WordNet and wordnets. In K. Brown (Ed.), Encyclopedia of language and linguistics, (2nd ed., pp. 665670). Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Gale, T.M., Irvine, K., Laws, K.R., Ferrissey, S. (2009). The naming profile in Alzheimer patients parallels that of elderly controls. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 31, 565574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilhooly, K.J., Logie, R.H. (1980). Age of acquisition, imagery, concreteness, familiarity and ambiguity measures for 1944 words. Behaviour Research, Methods, and Instruments, 12, 395427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T.H., Fennema-Notestine, C., Montoya, R.I., Jernigan, T.L. (2007). The bilingual effect on Boston Naming Test performance. The Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 197208.Google ScholarPubMed
Gollan, T.H., Salmon, D.P., Montoya, R.I., Da Pena, E. (2010). Accessibility of the nondominant language in picture naming: A counterintuitive effect of dementia on bilingual language production. Neuropsychologia, 48, 13561366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gollan, T.H., Weissberger, G.H., Runnqvist, E., Montoya, R.I., Cera, C.M. (2012). Self-ratings of spoken language dominance: A Multilingual Naming Test (MINT) and preliminary norms for young and aging Spanish-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 594615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, D.W. (1986). Control, activation and resource: A framework and a model for the control of speech in bilinguals. Brain and Language, 27, 210223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, D.W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 6781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodges, J.R., Salmon, D.P., Butters, N. (1991). The nature of the naming deficit in Alzheimer's and Huntington's disease. Brain, 114, 15471558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodges, J.R., Salmon, D.P., Butters, N. (1992). Semantic memory impairment in Alzheimer's disease: Failure of access or degraded knowledge? Neuropsychologia, 30, 301314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jescheniak, J.D., Levelt, W.J.M. (1994). Word frequency effects in speech production: Retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological form. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 20, 824843.Google Scholar
Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., Weintraub, S. (1983). The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia, PA: Lea & Febiger.Google Scholar
Kemmerer, D., Tranel, D. (2000). Verb retrieval in brain-damaged subjects: 1. Analysis of stimulus, lexical, and conceptual factors. Brain and Language, 73, 347–392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kittredge, A.K., Dell, G.S., Verkuilen, J., Schwartz, M.F. (2008). Where is the effect of frequency in word production? Insights from aphasic picture-naming errors. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 463492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohnert, K.J., Hernandez, A.E., Bates, E. (1998). Bilingual performance on the Boston Naming Test: Preliminary norms in Spanish and English. Brain and Language, 65, 422440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kremin, H., Perrier, D., De Wilde, M., Dordain, M., Le Bayon, A., Gatignol, P., Arabia, C. (2001). Factors predicting success in picture naming in Alzheimer's disease and primary progressive aphasia. Brain and Cognition, 46, 180183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lezak, M.D., Howieson, D.B., Loring, D.W. (2004). Neuropsychological Assessment. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D., Stadlan, E.M. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group. Neurology, 34, 939944.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, J.C., Weintraub, S., Chui, H.C., Cummings, J., Decarli, C., Ferris, S., Kukull, W.A. (2006). The Uniform Data Set (UDS): Clinical and cognitive variables and descriptive data from Alzheimer Disease Centers. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 20, 210216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nebes, R.D. (1989). Semantic memory in Alzheimer's disease. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 377394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oldfield, R.C., Wingfield, A. (1965). Response latencies in naming objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 17, 273281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paivio, A., Yuille, J.C., Madigan, S.A. (1968). Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. Journal of Experimental Psychology Monograph, 76, 125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, R.C., Smith, G.E., Waring, S.C., Ivnik, R.J., Tangalos, E.G., Kokmen, E. (1999). Mild cognitive impairment: Clinical characterization and outcome. Archives of Neurology, 56, 303308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plaut, D.C., Shallice, T. (1993). Deep dyslexia: A case study of connectionist neuropsychology. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 10, 377500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poulisse, N., Bongaerts, T. (1994). First language use in second language production. Applied Linguistics, 15, 3657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ransdell, S.E., Fischler, I. (1987). Memory in a monolingual mode: When are bilinguals at a disadvantage? Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 392405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, P.M., Garcia, L.J., Desrochers, A., Hernandez, D. (2002). English performance of proficient bilingual adults on the Boston Naming Test. Aphasiology, 16, 635645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salmon, D.P., Heindel, W.C., Lange, K.L. (1999). Differential decline in word generation from phonemic and semantic categories during the course of Alzheimer's disease: Implications for the integrity of semantic memory. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 5, 692703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spreen, O., Strauss, E. (1991). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms and commentary. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., Davis, C.J. (2006). The Bristol norms for age of acquisition, imageability, and familiarity. Behavior Research Methods, 38, 598605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Testa, J.A., Ivnik, R.J., Boeve, B., Petersen, R.C., Shane Pankratz, V., Knopman, D., Smith, G.E. (2004). Confrontation naming does not add incremental diagnostic utility in MCI and Alzheimer's disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 504512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson-Schill, S.L., Gabrieli, J.D., Fleischman, D.A. (1999). Effects of structural similarity and name frequency on picture naming in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 5, 659667.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toglia, M.P., Battig, W.R. (1978). Handbook of Semantic word norms. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar