Hostname: page-component-6b989bf9dc-pkhfk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-14T20:48:47.562Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A longitudinal study of factors predicting change in cognitive test scores over time, in an older hypertensive population

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2009

M. Prince*
Affiliation:
Section of Epidemiology and General Practice, Institute of Psychiatry and the Department of Epidemiology and Population Sciences, London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, London
G. Lewis
Affiliation:
Section of Epidemiology and General Practice, Institute of Psychiatry and the Department of Epidemiology and Population Sciences, London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, London
A. Bird
Affiliation:
Section of Epidemiology and General Practice, Institute of Psychiatry and the Department of Epidemiology and Population Sciences, London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, London
R. Blizard
Affiliation:
Section of Epidemiology and General Practice, Institute of Psychiatry and the Department of Epidemiology and Population Sciences, London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, London
A. Mann
Affiliation:
Section of Epidemiology and General Practice, Institute of Psychiatry and the Department of Epidemiology and Population Sciences, London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, London
*
1Address for correspondence: Dr Martin Prince, Section of Epidemiology and General Practice, Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF.

Synopsis

This study aims to describe factors associated with cognitive decline among 2584 subjects, aged 65–74, who were followed up for 54 months in the Medical Research Council Elderly Hypertension Trial (1982–1989). The subjects completed a cognitive test, the Paired Associate Learning Test (PALT), five times over this period. Decline on the PALT was associated with advanced age, male sex, rural residence, depression and low intelligence. These effects were modified by gender and level of pre-morbid intelligence. Advanced age, rural residence and number of cigarettes smoked daily were only associated with PALT decline among women of below median intelligence. The association between depression and PALT decline was only apparent in women of below median intelligence and men of above median intelligence. While these findings are consistent with other research into cognitive decline, they differ in some ways from reported risk factors for dementia, suggesting aetiological separateness. That women were more vulnerable than men to the effects of age and smoking raises the question of the impact on cognition of accelerated atherosclerosis after the menopause.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1996

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd edn Revised. APA: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Armitage, P. & Berry, G. (1987). Statistical Methods in Medical Research. Blackwell Scientific: Oxford.Google Scholar
Bassett, S. S. & Folstein, M. F. (1991). Cognitive impairment and functional disability in the absence of a psychiatric diagnosis. Psychological Medicine 21, 7784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bird, A. S., Macdonald, A. J. D., Mann, A. H. & Philpott, M. P. (1987). Preliminary experience with the SELF-CARE-D: a self-rating Depression Questionnaire for use with the elderly. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 2, 3138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bird, A. S., Blizard, R. A. & Mann, A. H. (1990). Treating hypertension in the older person: an evaluation of the association of blood pressure level and its reduction with cognitive performance. Journal of Hypertension 8, 147152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brayne, C. & Calloway, P. (1988). Normal ageing, impaired cognitive function, and senile dementia of the Alzheimer's types: a continuum? Lancet i, 12651267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breteler, M. M. B., Claus, J. J., Grobbee, D. E. & Hofman, A. (1994). Cardiovascular disease and distribution of cognitive function in elderly people: the Rotterdam study. British Medical Journal 308, 16041608.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chadwick, C. (1992). The MRC Multicentre Study of Cognitive Function and Ageing: a EURODEM incidence study in progress. Neuroepidemiology 11 (suppl 1), 3743.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cockburn, J. & Smith, P. T. (1991). The relative influence of intelligence and age on everyday memory. Journal of Gerontology 46. P3136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colsher, L. P. & Wallace, R. B. (1989). Data quality and age: health and psychobehavioural correlates of item nonresponse and inconsistent responses. Journal of Gerontology 44, P4552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colsher, L. P. & Wallace, R. B. (1991). Longitudinal application of cognitive function measures in a defined population of community-dwelling elders. Annals of Epidemiology 1, 215230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craik, F. I. M., Byrd, M. & Swanson, J. M. (1987). Patterns of memory loss in three elderly samples. Psychology and Aging 2, 7986.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deeg, D. J., Hofman, A. & Van Zonneveld, R. J. (1990). The association between change in cognitive function and longevity in the elderly. American Journal of Epidemiology 132, 973982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elias, M. F., Wolf, P. A., D'Agostino, R. B., Cobb, J. & White, L. R. (1993). Untreated blood pressure is inversely related to cognitive functioning: the Framingham Study. American Journal of Epidemiology 138, 353364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, D. A. (1991). Why should we study change in cognitive function? Annals of Epidemiology 1, 283284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farmer, M. E., White, L. R., Abbott, R. D., Kittner, S. J., Kaplan, E., Wolz, M. M., Brody, J. A. & Wolf, P. A. (1987). Blood pressure and cognitive performance: the Framingham Study. American Journal of Epidemiology 126, 11031114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farmer, M. E., Kittner, S. J., Abbott, R. D., Wolz, M. M., Wolf, P. A. & White, L. R. (1990). Longitudinally measured blood pressure, antihypertensive medication use, and cognitive performance: the Framingham Study. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 43. 475480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feskens, E. J. M., Havekes, L. M., Kalmijn, S., de Knijff, P., Launer, L. J. & Kromhout, D. (1994). Apolipoprotein E4 allele and cognitive decline in elderly men. British Medical Journal 309, 12021206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fratiglioni, L., Jorm, A. F., Grut, M., Viitanen, M., Holmen, K., Ahlbom, A. & Winblad, B. (1993). Predicting dementia from the Mini-Mental State Examination in an elderly population: the role of education. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 46, 281287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuhrer, R., Antonucci, T. C., Gagnon, M., Dartigues, J. F., Barbeger Gateau, P. & Alperovitch, A. (1992). Depressive symptomatology and cognitive functioning: an epidemiological survey in an elderly community sample in France. Psychological Medicine 22, 159172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gurland, B. J., Copeland, J., Kuriansky, J., Kelleher, M., Sharpe, L. & Dean, L. (1983). The Mind and Mood of Aging. Croom Helm: London.Google Scholar
Hebert, L. E., Scherr, P. A., Beckett, L. A., Albert, M. S., Rosner, B., Taylor, J. O. & Evans, D. A. (1993). Relation of smoking and low-to-moderate alcohol consumption to change in cognitive function: a longitudinal study in a defined community of older persons. American Journal of Epidemiology 137, 881891.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inglis, J. (1959). A paired-associate learning test for use with elderly psychiatric patients. Journal of Menial Science 105, 440443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jagger, C. & Clarke, M. (1988). Mortality risks in the elderly: five year follow-up of a total population. International Journal of Epidemiology 17, 111114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorm, A. F., Korten, A. E. & Henderson, A. S. (1987). The prevalence of dementia: a quantitative integration of the literature. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 76, 465479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kay, D. W. K., Henderson, A. S., Scott, R., Wilson, J., Rickwood, D. & Grayson, D. A. (1985). Dementia and depression among the elderly living in the Hobart community: the effect of diagnostic criteria on prevalence rates. Psychological Medicine 15, 771788.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuriansky, J., Gurland, B. & Cowan, D. (1976). The usefulness of a psychological test battery. International Journal of Aging and Human Development 7, 331342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Launer, L. J. (1992). Overview of incidence studies of dementia conducted in Europe. Neuroepidemiology 11 (suppl 1), 213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Little, A., Hemsley, D., Bergmann, K., Volans, J. & Levy, R. (1987). Comparison of the sensitivity of three instruments for the detection of cognitive decline in elderly living at home. British Journal of Psychiatry 150, 808814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, I. Y., Lacroix, A. Z., White, L. R., Kittner, S. J. & Wolf, P. A. (1990). Cognitive impairment and mortality: a study of possible confounders. American Journal of Epidemiology 132, 136143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D. & Stadlan, E. M. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease. Neurology 34, 939944.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, J. N. S., Altman, D. G., Campbell, M. J. & Royston, P. (1990). Analysis of serial measures in medical research. British Medical Journal 300, 230235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MRC Working Party. (1992). MRC trial of treatment of hypertension in older adults: principal results. British Medical Journal 304, 405412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, H. & O'Connell, A. (1978). Dementia. The estimation of premorbid intelligence levels using the New Adult Reading Test. Cortex 14, 234244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osterweil, D., Syndulko, K., Cohen, S. N., Pettler-Jennings, P. D., Hershmann, J. M., Cummings, J. L., Tourtelotte, W. W. & Solomon, D. H. (1992). Cognitive function in non-demented adults with hypothyroidism. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 40, 325335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perlmutter, M. (1978). What is memory aging the aging of? Developmental Psychology 14, 330345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prince, M. J., Cullen, M. C. & Mann, A. H. (1994). Risk factors for Alzheimer's disease and dementia: a case-control study based on the MRC Elderly Hypertension Trial. Neurology 44, 97104.Google ScholarPubMed
Raven, J. C. (1940). Matrix tests. Mental Health 1, 1018.Google ScholarPubMed
Reltan, R. M. (1959). Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indication of organic brain damage. Perceptual Motor Skills 8, 271276.Google Scholar
Sands, L. P. & Meredith, W. (1992). Blood pressure and intellectual functioning in late midlife. Journal of Gerontology 47, P8184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saunders, A. M., Strittmatter, W. J., Schmechel, D., St. George-Hyslop, P. H., Pericak-Vance, M. A., Joo, S. H., Rosi, B. L., Gusella, J. F., Crapper-Maclachlan, D. R., Alberts, M. J., Hulette, C., Crain, B., Goldgaber, D. & Roses, A. D. (1993). Association of apolipoprotein E allele e4 with late-onset familial and sporadic Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 43, 14671472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaie, K. W. (1983). The Seattle Longitudinal Study: a 21 year exploration of psychometric intelligence in adulthood. In Longitudinal Studies of Adult Psychological Development (ed. Schaie, K. W.), pp. 64135. Guilford Press: New York.Google Scholar
Scherr, P. A., Albert, M. S., Funkenstein, H. H., Cook, N. R., Hennekens, C. H., Branch, L. G., White, L. R., Taylor, J. O. & Evans, D. A. (1988). Correlates of cognitive function in an elderly community population. American Journal of Epidemiology 128, 10841101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schultz, N. R., Elias, M. F., Robbins, M. A., Streeten, M. H. P. & Blakeman, N. (1989). A longitudinal study of the performance of hypertensive and normotensive subjects on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. Psychology and Aging 4, 495499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shock, N. W. (1985). Longitudinal studies of aging in humans. In Handbook of the Biology of Aging, (ed. Finch, C. E. and Schneider, E. L.), pp. 721743. Van Nostrand Reinhold: New York.Google Scholar
Siegler, I. C. (1983). Psychological aspects of the Duke Longitudinal Studies. In Longitudinal Studies of Adult Psychological Development (ed. Schaie, K. W.), pp. 136190. Guilford Press: New York.Google Scholar
Swan, G. E., LaRue, A., Carmelli, D., Reed, T. & Fabsitz, R. R. (1992). Decline in cognitive performance and aging twins. Heritability and biobehavioural predictors from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute Twin Study. Archives of Neurology 49, 476481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilkie, F. & Eisdorfer, C. (1971). Intelligence and blood pressure in the aged. Science 172, 959962.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed