Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-cfpbc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T18:05:07.467Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sex and Games: On Oppression and Rationality

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 January 2009

Extract

‘Prejudice apart, the game of push-pin is of equal value with the arts and sciences of music and poetry’, was penned by Jeremy Bentham in 1825, but could as appropriately appear in most contemporary treatments of social choice and rationality. Political economists still use the felicific calculus; they search for ‘the greatest happiness of the greatest number’ by exploring means and consequences of aggregating individuals' preferences to form social choices among competing values. The values are assigned equal weight. The origins and processes of development of individual preferences are taken as ‘givens’, i.e. as irrelevant to the problem of social choice. What counts is rational behaviour, which is said to exist when action is ‘correctly’ designed to maximize goal achievement, ‘given the goal in question and the real world as it exists’.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1979

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

1 Bentham, Jeremy, The Rationale of Reward, 1825Google Scholar; see Bowring, John, ed., The Works of Jeremy Bentham, Vol. II (Edinburgh: William Tait, 18381843), Book III, p. 253b.Google Scholar

2 Dahl, Robert A. and Lindblom, Charles, Politics, Economics, and Welfare (New York: Harper and Row, 1963), p. 38.Google Scholar

3 Halevy, Elie, The Philosophic Radicals (Boston, Mass.: Beacon, 1955).Google Scholar

4 Laing, R. D., Knots (New York: Random House, 1970), p. 5.Google Scholar

5 The internal dimension of oppression is available through one of the obsolete meanings of the word. Many women argue – in the face of considerable resistance – that rape is a form of oppression used by males against females. As the Oxford English Dictionary tells us, one of the oldest meanings of the verb ‘to oppress’ is ‘to rape’.

6 On the internalization of stereotype, see, for example, Deaux, Kay, The Behavior of Women and Men (Monterey, Calif.: Wadsworth, 1976)Google Scholar; Fanon, Frantz, The Wretched of the Earth (New York: Grove Press, 1966)Google Scholar; Hacker, Helen Meyer, ‘Women as a Minority Group’, Social Forces, xxx (1951), 60–9CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Kardiner, Abram and Ovesey, Lionel, The Mark of Oppression: Explorations in the Personality of the American Negro (Cleveland, Ohio: World, 1951)Google Scholar; and Allport, Gordon W., The Nature of Prejudice (Cambridge, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1954).Google Scholar

7 Allport, , The Nature of Prejudice.Google Scholar

8 Fanon, , The Wretched of the Earth, p. 61.Google Scholar

9 On self-esteem among American blacks, see Deutsch, Martin, Katz, Irwin, and Jensen, Arthur R., eds., Social Class, Race, and Psychological Development (New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1968)Google Scholar and Kardiner, and Ovesey, , The Mark of OppressionGoogle Scholar. On women, see Bardwick, Judith, Psychology of Women: A Study of Bio-Cultural Conflicts (New York: Harper and Row, 1971)Google Scholar and Deaux, , The Behavior of Women and MenGoogle Scholar. On colonized populations, see Fanon, , The Wretched of the Earth.Google Scholar

10 For an excellent example of this, see Hoffman, Lois Wladis, ‘Fear of Success in 1965 and 1974: A Follow-Up Study’, Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, XLV (1977), 310–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar. See also Goldberg, Philip A., ‘Are Women Prejudiced against Women?’, Transaction, v (1968), 2830Google Scholar and Pheterson, Gail, Keisler, Sara and Goldberg, Philip, ‘Evaluation of the Performance of Women as a Function of Their Sex, Achievement Level, and Personal History’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, xix (1971), 114–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

11 Tormey, Judith, ‘Exploitation, Oppression, and Self-Sacrifice’, in Gould, Carol and Wartofsky, Marx, eds., Women and Philosophy (New York: Putnam, 1976), p. 217.Google Scholar

12 Wolfe, Alan, The Seamy Side of Democracy (New York: Longman, 1973), p. 6.Google Scholar

13 de Beauvoir, Simone, The Second Sex (New York: Knopf, 1952), p. xxi.Google Scholar

14 Zolberg, Aristide, ‘Moments of Madness’Google Scholar, in Katznelson, Ira et al. , The Politics and Society Reader (New York: Longman, 1974), pp. 232–56.Google Scholar

15 Fanon, , The Wretched of the Earth, p. 192.Google Scholar

16 On ‘irrationality’ among American blacks, see Kardiner, and Ovesey, , The Mark of Oppression.Google Scholar

17 For example, although more men than women commit suicide, women outnumber men in attempts by four to one. For a review of this perspective on female psychological disorders see Marecek, Jeanne, ‘Psychological Disorders in Women: Indices of Role Strain’Google Scholar, in Frieze, Kene H. et al. , Women and Sex Roles: A Social Psychological Perspective (New York: Norton, 1978), pp. 255–76.Google Scholar

18 Laing, , Knots, p. 5.Google Scholar

19 The following is a necessarily scant outline of part of Wollstonecraft's observations on the effects of oppression. Most of her life's work was devoted to this theme and obviously cannot be summarized adequately in a few pages. Moreover, she reserved some important parts of her theory and observations for a volume to follow the Vindication of the Rights of Woman (Baltimore: Penguin, 1975)Google Scholar. This volume never appeared, probably because she died as a result of childbirth five years later at the age of thirty-eight. For more on Wollstonecraft's life and work, see Sapiro, Virginia, ‘Feminist Studies and the Discipline: A Study of Mary Wollstonecraft’, Michigan Papers in Women's Studies, i (1974), 178200Google Scholar and Tomalin, Claire, The Life and Death of Mary Wollstonecraft (New York: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovitch, 1974)Google Scholar. The latest collection of her major works is found in Todd, Janet M., ed., A Wollstonecraft Anthology (Bloomington: Indiana University Press, 1977).Google Scholar

20 Wollstonecraft, , A Vindication of the Rights of Woman, p. 85Google Scholar. Further references to this work will be indicated by page numbers inserted in the text.

21 ‘Madness is only the absence of reason… the ruling angel leaving its seat, wild anarchy ensues’, Wollstonecraft, Mary, A Vindication of the Rights of Men (Gainesville: Scholar's Facsimile, 1960), p. 64Google Scholar. ‘It is a proverbial observation, that a very thin partition divides wit and madness’, Wollstonecraft, , A Vindication of the Rights of Men, p. 64.Google Scholar

22 Wollstonecraft, , A Vindication of the Rights of Men, p. 28.Google Scholar

23 For a discussion of these forms of behaviour among peasants, see Scott, James C., The Moral Economy of the Peasant: Rebellion and Subsistence in Southeast Asia (New Haven, Conn.: Yale University Press, 1976), especially pp. 231–40.Google Scholar

24 It is important to note that excessive present-orientation and the corrollaries Wollstonecraft draws are integral parts of the stereotypes assigned to many Third World colonial populations as well as a number of minority groups in the United States.

25 Wollstonecraft, , A Vindication of the Rights of Men, p. 92.Google Scholar

26 For example, Searing, Donald and Stern, Alan, ‘The Stratification Beliefs of British and American Adolescents’, British Journal of Political Science, vi (1976), 177203.Google Scholar

27 For accessible discussions of the utility of game theory and game studies, see Kramer, Gerald H. and Hertzberg, Joseph, ‘Formal Theory’, in Greenstein, Fred I. and Polsby, Nelson, eds., Handbook of Political Science, Vol. VII (Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1975), pp. 351403Google Scholar, and Schlenker, Barry R. and Bonoma, Thomas V., ‘Fun and Games: The Validity of Games for the Study of Conflict’, Journal of Conflict Resolution, xxii (1978), 738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

28 For a typical sentiment, ‘Finally, it is heartening to note, contrary to the suggestions… that ‘chivalry is dead’, that being paired with a female immediately predisposed the Canadian males to behave in a co-operative manner. Traditional sex roles still have some effect’, Carment, D. W., ‘Effects of Sex Role in a Maximizing Difference Game’, Journal of Conflict Resolution, xviii (1974), 461–72, p. 471 (italics added).CrossRefGoogle Scholar

29 Riker, William H. and Zavoina, William James, ‘Rational Behavior in Politics: Evidence from a Three Person Game’, American Political Science Review, LXIV (1970), 4860, p. 56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

30 Riker, and Zavoina, , ‘Rational Behavior in Politics’, p. 56.Google Scholar

31 Riker, and Zavoina, , ‘Rational Behavior in Politics’, p. 57.Google Scholar

32 Riker, and Zavoina, , ‘Rational Behavior in Politics’, p. 58.Google Scholar

33 All interpretations of the results of these studies are mine unless otherwise indicated. There is one caveat to bear in mind in attempting to collate, discuss, and read experimental game literature. These studies together constitute perhaps the clearest example of ‘normal science’ in the literature of social science. There are scores of studies relating to the points discussed here; most arc largely replications of previous studies, performed within the rubric of ‘game theory’. However, each differs at least slightly from each of the others. The pay-off matrices, sex of experimenter, introductory stimulus, and any number of other factors are unique to the individual study. Thus, complete comparability is not possible. Moreover, for those who have not sampled the literature on experimental game studies, it is most difficult to discuss it in clear prose free from jargon. I have attempted to write in English wherever possible.

34 For a brief explanation of Prisoner's Dilemma Games see Appendix.

35 See Bedell, Jeffrey and Sistrunk, Frank, ‘Power, Opportunity Losses, and Sex in a Mixed-Motive Game’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, xxv (1973), 219–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Bixenstine, V. Edwin, Chambers, Norman, and Wilson, Kellog V., ‘Effect of Assymetry in Payoff Behavior in a Two-Person, Non-Zero Sum Game’, Journal of Conflict Resolution, viii (1964), 151–9CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Carment, , ‘Effects of Sex Role in a Maximizing Difference Game’Google Scholar; Kahn, Arnold, Hottes, Joe and Davis, William L., ‘Cooperation and Optimal Responding in the Prisoner's Dilemma Game: Effects of Sex and Physical Attractiveness’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, XVII (1971), 267–79CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Komorita, S. S., ‘Cooperative Choice in a Prisoner's Dilemma Game’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, xxi (1965), 741–5CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Mack, David, Auburn, Paula and Knight, George P., ‘Sex Role, Identification, and Behavior in a Reiterated Prisoner's Dilemma Game’, Psychonomic Science, xxiv (1971), 280–1CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Marwell, G., Schmitt, D. and Shotola, R., ‘Sex Differences in a Cooperative Task’, Behavioral Science, xiv (1970), 184–6CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Oskamp, Stuart and Perlman, Daniel, ‘Factors Affecting Cooperation in a Prisoner's Dilemma Game’, Journal of Conflict Resolution, ix (1965), 359–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Rapoport, Anatol and Chammah, Albert, ‘Sex Differences in Factors Contributing to the Level of Cooperation in Prisoner's Dilemma Game’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 11 (1965), 831–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar; and Steele, M. and Tedeschi, J. T., ‘Matrix Indices and Strategy Choices in Mixed-Motive Games’, Journal of Conflict Resolution, xi (1967), 189205.Google Scholar

36 Rapoport, and Chammah, , ‘Sex Differences in Factors Contributing to the Level of Cooperation’Google Scholar; Garment, , ‘Effects of Sex Role in a Maximizing Difference Game’Google Scholar, Mack, et al. , ‘Sex Role, Identification, and Behavior’.Google Scholar

37 Bixenstine, et al. , ‘Effect of Assymetry in Payoff Behavior’, p. 157Google Scholar. As we shall see below, women are more forgiving if the opponent is male.

38 Mack, et al. , ‘Sex Role, Identification, and Behavior’, p. 201.Google Scholar

39 For other examples of ‘personalism’ in bargaining and exchange within stratified systems see Scott, , The Moral Economy of the PeasantGoogle Scholar and Genovese, Eugene D., Roll, Jordan, Roll: The World the Slaves Made (New York: Pantheon, 1974).Google Scholar

40 Bixenstine, et al. , ‘Effect of Assymetry in Payoff Behavior’, p. 157.Google Scholar

41 See Aranoff, Daniel and Tedeschi, James T., ‘Original Stakes and Behavior in the Prisoner's Dilemma Game’, Psychonomic Science, xii (1968), 7980CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Evans, Gary and Crumbaugh, Charles M., ‘Payment Schedule, Sequence Choice, and Cooperation in the Prisoner's Dilemma Game’, Psychonomic Science, v (1966), 87–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar; McClintock, C. G., Gallo, P. and Harrison, A., ‘Some Effects of Variations in Other's Strategy upon Game Behavior’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, xxi (1965), 319–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar; McNeel, Stephen P., McClintock, Charles G. and Nuttin, Jozef M., ‘Effects of Sex Role in a Two-Person Mixed Motive Game’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, xxiv (1972), 372–80CrossRefGoogle Scholar; and Wiley, Mary Glenn, ‘Sex Roles in Games’, Sociometry, xxxvi (1973), 526–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

42 Aranoff, and Tedeschi, , ‘Original Stakes and Behavior in the Prisoner's Dilemma Game’Google Scholar; Lindskold, Svenn, McElwain, Douglas C. and Wayner, Marc, ‘Cooperation and the Use of Coercion by Groups of Individuals’, Journal of Conflict Resolution, xxi (1971), 531–50.Google Scholar

43 Aranoff, and Tedeschi, , ‘Original Stakes and Behavior in the Prisoner's Dilemma Game.’Google Scholar

44 Fischer, Gloria J., ‘Probability Learning and Attitude toward Women as a Function of Monetary Risk, Gain, and Sex’, Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, ix (1977), 201–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

45 Hottes, Joseph H. and Kahn, Arnold, ‘Sex Differences in a Mixed-Motive Conflict Situation’, Journal of Personality, XLII (1974), 260–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Komorita, , ‘Cooperative Choice in a Prisoner's Dilemma Game’.Google Scholar

46 Lindskold, , McElwain, and Wayner, , ‘Cooperation and the Use of Coercion by Groups and Individuals’.Google Scholar

47 Wiley, , ‘Sex Roles and Games’.Google Scholar

48 See also Black, Terry E. and Higbee, Kenneth L., ‘Effects of Power, Threat, and Sex on Exploitation’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, xxvii (1973), 382–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

49 For a full length treatment of risk of loss by dependents see Scott, , The Moral Economy of the Peasant.Google Scholar

50 McNeel, et al. , ‘Effects of Sex in a Two-Person Mixed Motive Game’.Google Scholar

51 Evans, and Crumbaugh, , ‘Payment Schedule, Sequence Choices, and Cooperation’.Google Scholar

52 Wiley, , ‘Sex Roles and Games’.Google Scholar

53 Skotko, Vincent, Langmeyer, Daniel and Lundgren, David, ‘Sex Differences as Artifact in the Prisoner's Dilemma Game’, Journal of Conflict Resolution, xviii (1974), 707–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

54 One study, using only male subjects, found significant results. See Deutsch, Morton, Canavan, Donna and Rubin, Jeffrey, ‘The Effects of Size of Conflict and Sex of Experimenter upon Interpersonal Bargaining’, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, vii (1971), 258–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

55 Fischer, , ‘Probability Learning and Attitude toward Women.’Google Scholar

56 Wiley, , ‘Sex Roles and Games’Google Scholar, and McNeel, et al. , ‘Effects of Sex in a Two-Person Mixed Motive Game’.Google Scholar

57 McNeel, et al. , ‘Effects of Sex in a Two-Person Mixed Motive Game’.Google Scholar

58 Bedell, and Sistrunk, , ‘Power, Opportunity Losses, and Sex’Google Scholar; Skotko, et al. , ‘Sex Differences as Artifact’.Google Scholar

59 Kahn, et al. , ‘Cooperation and Optimal Responding in the Prisoner's Dilemma Game’.Google Scholar

60 Carment, , ‘Effects of Sex Role in a Maxmizing Difference Game’Google Scholar, Mack, et al. , ‘Sex Role, Identification, and Behavior’.Google Scholar

61 Carment, , ‘Effects of Sex Role in a Maximizing Difference Game’.Google Scholar

62 Wiley, , ‘Sex Roles in Games’, p. 538.Google Scholar

63 Carment, , ‘Effects of Sex Role in a Maximizing Difference Game’Google Scholar, McNeel, et al. , ‘Effects of Sex in a Two-Person Mixed Motive Game’Google Scholar; Wiley, , ‘Sex Roles in Games’.Google Scholar

64 Wyer, Robert S. and Malinowski, Christine, ‘Effects of Sex and Achievement Level upon Individualism and Competitiveness in Social Interaction’, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, viii (1972), 303–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

65 Wyer, and Malinowski, , ‘Effects of Sex and Achievement Level’, p. 312.Google Scholar

66 Wyer, and Malinowski, , ‘Effects of Sex and Achievement Level’, p. 313.Google Scholar

67 McNeel, et al. , ‘Effects of Sex in a Two-Person Mixed Motive Game’.Google Scholar

68 Wyer, and Malinowski, , ‘Effects of Sex and Achievement Level’.Google Scholar

69 Reported in Deaux, , The Behavior of Women and Men, pp. 88–9.Google Scholar

70 Lindskold, et al. , ‘Cooperation and the Use of Coercion’.Google Scholar

71 Halpin, S. M. and Pilisuk, M., ‘Prediction and Choice in the Prisoner's Dilemma’, Behavioral Science, xv (1970), 141–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

72 Borah, L. A., ‘Effects of Threat in Bargaining: Critical and Experimental Analysis’, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, LXVI (1963), 3744CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Shomer, Robert W., Davis, Alice H. and Kelley, Harold H., ‘Threats and the Development of Coordination: Further Studies of the Deutsch and Krauss Trucking Game’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, iv (1966), 119–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

73 Reported in Ingram, Barbara Lichner and Berger, Stephen E., ‘Sex-Role Orientation, Defensiveness, and Competitiveness in Women’, Journal of Conflict Resolution, xxi (1977), 401–18.Google Scholar

74 Baefsky, P. M. and Berger, S. E., ‘Self-Sacrifice, Cooperation, and Aggression in Women of Varying Sex-Role Orientations’, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 1 (1974), 296–8.Google Scholar

75 Ingram, and Berger, , ‘Sex-Role Orientation, Defensiveness, and Competitiveness’.Google Scholar

76 Wollstonecraft, , The Vindication of the Rights of Woman, p. 83.Google Scholar

77 See Feather, N. T. and Raphelson, Alfred C., ‘Fear of Success in Australian and American Student Groups: Motive or Sex-Role Stereotype?Journal of Personality, XLII (1974), 190201CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Hoffman, , ‘Fear of Success’Google Scholar; Horner, Matina S., ‘Toward an Understanding of Achievement-Related Conflicts in Women’, Journal of Social Issues, xxviii (1972), 157–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Monahan, Lynn, Kuhn, Deanna and Shaver, Phillip, ‘Intrapsychic versus Cultural Explanations of the “Fear of Success” Motive’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, xxix (1974), 5064Google Scholar; Stein, Aletha Huston and Bailey, Margaret M., ‘The Socialization of Achievement Orientation in Females, Psychological Bulletin, LXXX (1973), 345–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

78 Hoffman, , ‘Fear of Success’.Google Scholar

79 Deaux, Kay and Emswiller, Tim, ‘Explanation of Successful Performance on Sex Linked Tasks: What Is Skill for the Male Is Luck for the Female’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, xxix (1974), 80–5CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Feather, N. T. and Simon, J. G., ‘Reactions to Male and Female Success and Failure in Sex-Linked Occupations: Impressions of Personality, Causal Attributions, and Perceived Likelihood of Different Consequences’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, xxxi (1974), 2031Google Scholar; Feldman-Summers, Shirley and Kiesler, Sara B., ‘Those Who Are Number Two Try Harder: The Effect of Sex on Attributions of Causality’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, xxx (1974), 846–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

80 Deaux, , The Behavior of Women and Men, p. 42.Google Scholar

81 Leventhal, Gerald S. and Lane, Douglas W., ‘Sex, Age, and Equity Behavior’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, xv (1970), 312–16CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Mikula, G., ‘Nationality, Performance, and Sex as Determinants of Reward Allocation’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, xxix (1974), 435–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

82 Stein, Aletha Huston, Pohly, Shiela Rimland and Mueller, Edward, ‘The Influence of Masculine, Feminine, and Natural Tasks on Children's Achievement Behavior, Expectancies of Success, and Attainment Values’, Child Development, XLII (1971), 195207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

83 Riker, and Zavoina, , ‘Rational Behavior in Politics’.Google Scholar

84 Riker, William H. and Ordeshook, Peter C., An Introduction to Positive Political Theory (Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall, 1973), p. 10.Google Scholar

85 Laing, , Knots, p. 5.Google Scholar

86 For discussions of love and oppression, see Rapaport, Elizabeth, ‘On the Future of Love: Rousseau and the Radical Feminists’Google Scholar, in Gould, and Wartofsky, , Women and Philosophy, pp. 185205Google Scholar, and Tormey, , ‘Exploitation, Oppression, and Self-Sacrifice.’Google Scholar

87 ‘Some will object, that a comparison cannot fairly be made between the government of the male sex and the forms of unjust power which I have adduced in illustration of it, since these are arbitrary, and the effect of mere usurpation, while it on the contrary is natural. But was there ever any domination which did not appear natural to those who possessed it?’ Mill, John Stuart, ‘The Subjection of Women’, in Rossi, Alice, ed., Essays in Sex Equality (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1970), p. 147.Google Scholar

88 As presented in Brams, Steven J., Paradoxes in Politics: An Introduction to the Nonobvious in Political Science (New York: Macmillan, 1976), p. 82.Google Scholar

89 For a more technical discussion of the Prisoner's Dilemma Game, see Rapoport, Anatol and Chammah, Albert M., Prisoner's Dilemma: A Study in Conflict and Cooperation (Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press, 1965).CrossRefGoogle Scholar