Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-16T14:04:53.171Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Associations between modifiable lifestyle factors and multidimensional cognitive health among community-dwelling old adults: stratified by educational level

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 February 2018

Manqiong Yuan
Affiliation:
State Key Laboratory of Molecular Vaccinology and Molecular Diagnostics, School of Public Health, Xiamen University, Xiamen 361102, China Key Laboratory of Health Technology Assessment of Fujian Province, School of Public Health, Xiamen University, Xiamen 361102, China
Jia Chen
Affiliation:
State Key Laboratory of Molecular Vaccinology and Molecular Diagnostics, School of Public Health, Xiamen University, Xiamen 361102, China Key Laboratory of Health Technology Assessment of Fujian Province, School of Public Health, Xiamen University, Xiamen 361102, China
Yaofeng Han
Affiliation:
State Key Laboratory of Molecular Vaccinology and Molecular Diagnostics, School of Public Health, Xiamen University, Xiamen 361102, China Key Laboratory of Health Technology Assessment of Fujian Province, School of Public Health, Xiamen University, Xiamen 361102, China
Xingliang Wei
Affiliation:
State Key Laboratory of Molecular Vaccinology and Molecular Diagnostics, School of Public Health, Xiamen University, Xiamen 361102, China
Zirong Ye
Affiliation:
State Key Laboratory of Molecular Vaccinology and Molecular Diagnostics, School of Public Health, Xiamen University, Xiamen 361102, China Key Laboratory of Health Technology Assessment of Fujian Province, School of Public Health, Xiamen University, Xiamen 361102, China
Liangwen Zhang
Affiliation:
State Key Laboratory of Molecular Vaccinology and Molecular Diagnostics, School of Public Health, Xiamen University, Xiamen 361102, China Key Laboratory of Health Technology Assessment of Fujian Province, School of Public Health, Xiamen University, Xiamen 361102, China
Y. Alicia Hong
Affiliation:
School of Public Health, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, USA
Ya Fang*
Affiliation:
State Key Laboratory of Molecular Vaccinology and Molecular Diagnostics, School of Public Health, Xiamen University, Xiamen 361102, China Key Laboratory of Health Technology Assessment of Fujian Province, School of Public Health, Xiamen University, Xiamen 361102, China
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Ya Fang, Prof., School of Public Health, Xiamen University, Xiang'an Nan Road, Xiang'an District, Xiamen, Fujian 361102, China. Phone: (+86)0592-2880636; Fax: (+86)0592-2880639. Email: fangya@xmu.edu.cn.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background:

Cognition is multidimensional, and each domain plays a unique and crucial part in successful daily life engagement. However, less attention has been paid to multi-domain cognitive health for the elderly, and the role of lifestyle factors in each domain remains unclear.

Methods:

We conducted a cross-sectional study of 3,230 older adults aged 60+ years in Xiamen, China, in 2016. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) was used to measure general cognition and six specific sub-domains. To account for educational effects, we adjusted the MoCA score and divided respondents into three education-specific groups (low, moderate, and high education groups with ≤5, 6~8, and ≥9 years of education, respectively). A series of proportional odds models were used to detect the associations between two categories of lifestyle factors – substance abuse (cigarette and alcohol) and leisure activity (TV watching, reading, smartphone use, social activity, and exercise) – and general cognition and the six sub-domains in those three groups.

Results:

Among the 3,230 respondents, 2,617 eligible participants were included with a mean age of 69.05 ± 7.07 years. Previous or current smoking/drinking was not associated with MoCA scores in the whole population, but unexpectedly, the ex-smokers in the low education group performed better in general cognition (OR = 2.22) and attention (OR = 2.05) than their never-smoking counterparts. Modest TV watching, reading, and smartphone use also contributed to better cognition among elderly participants in the low education group. For the highly educated elderly, comparatively longer reading (>3.5 hours/week) was inversely associated with general cognition (OR = 0.53), memory (OR = 0.59), and language (OR = 0.54), while adequate exercise (5~7 days/week) was positively related to these factors with OR = 1.48, OR = 1.49, and OR = 1.53, respectively. For the moderately educated elderly, only modest reading was significantly beneficial.

Conclusions:

Lifestyle factors play different roles in multidimensional cognitive health in different educational groups, indicating that individual intervention strategies should be designed according to specific educational groups and different cognitive sub-domains.

Type
Original Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © International Psychogeriatric Association 2018

Introduction

Population aging is considered one of the most significant social problems around the world today. According to data from the Department of Economic and Social Affairs of the United Nations, 12.3% of people worldwide (900 million) were over the age of 60 years in 2015, and this figure is projected to increase by 16.5% by 2030 (1.40 billion). Alzheimer's disease, as an aging-associated disease, has consistently aroused public awareness worldwide for not only affecting individuals’ quality of life but also imposing substantial financial burdens on society and emotional burdens on caregivers (Callahan, Reference Callahan2017). However, less attention has been paid to cognitive decline per se, particularly from a public health perspective. It is reported that numerous age-related changes in cognition are highly relevant to daily activities and have considerable importance to the public (IOM, 2015).

Cognition is multidimensional, encompassing processes such as attention, memory, executive functioning, logic, and language function. Collectively, these different domains of cognition play a fundamental and crucial part in successful engagement in daily life. For example, declines in memory may influence people's adherence to a medication schedule (Insel et al., Reference Insel, Morrow, Brewer and Figueredo2006), deterioration in executive functioning is linked to declines in ability to perform instrumental activities of daily living (Bell-McGinty et al., Reference Bell-McGinty, Podell, Franzen, Baird and Williams2002), and other degenerations in cognitive abilities can increase the risk of making errors in financial decisions (Samanez-Larkin et al., Reference Samanez-Larkin, Levens, Perry, Dougherty and Knutson2012) and damaging performance on technology-based tasks such as searching the internet (Czaja et al., Reference Czaja2013). Hence, maintaining one's cognition in different domains is quite important to assure the quality of one's late life. Furthermore, it is quite necessary to gain a deep understanding of the beneficial and deleterious factors affecting cognition in different domains and to have a prevention guide.

Cognitive decline is considered to be multifactorial, with unmodifiable factors (e.g. age and genetics) and modifiable factors (e.g. environmental factors and lifestyle factors) (Ngandu, Reference Ngandu2006). Regarding the accessibility and modifiability of individuals’ behaviors, a growing number of studies were conducted to underline the association of modifiable lifestyle factors with dementia (Baumgart et al., Reference Baumgart, Snyder, Carrillo, Fazio, Kim and Johns2015). For example, abuse of substances such as cigarettes (Ohara et al., Reference Ohara2015) and alcohol (Ormstad et al., Reference Ormstad, Rosness, Bergem, Bjertness, Strand and Group2016) and leisure activities such as TV watching (Shin et al., Reference Shin2013), reading (Lopes et al., Reference Lopes, Ferrioli, Nakano, Litvoc and Bottino2012), exercise (Wong et al., Reference Wong2016), and social activity (Wong et al., Reference Wong2016) have been studied substantially for their associations with dementia. However, little is known about their associations with each specific domain of cognitive function. In particular, smartphones, a very popular device currently, had been reported to have an independent facilitating effect on global cognition among old people (Ng et al., Reference Ng, Lim, Niti and Collinson2012); hence, we included it and sought to detect its associations with different cognitive domains.

In this study, we aimed to understand the role of lifestyle factors – substance abuse (cigarettes and alcohol) and leisure activity (TV watching, reading, smartphone use, exercise, and social activity) – in cognitive health, particularly in different domains of cognitive function, and to guide individual interventions for specific kinds of decline in cognitive domains.

Methods

Participants

A cross-sectional study was conducted with individuals who were registered residents aged over 60 years from July 1st to October 20th in 2016. To create a baseline database, we proposed to cover 1% of overall registered individuals aged over 60 years in Xiamen, where there were 275.8 thousand eligible participants at the time of this survey. Hence, the calculated sample size was 2,758 individuals. Given a non-response rate of 15%, the sample size was inflated to 3,172.

A multistage random sampling method was used. The first stage of sampling involved all six districts in Xiamen, including two urban and four rural districts. Half of the sub-districts were randomly selected from each district in the second stage, and 44 communities in total were randomly chosen in the third stage of sampling. In the fourth stage, individuals were randomly selected in each community by controlling for gender and age, and the sampling size was determined according to its proportion of eligible older adults. Participants were excluded if they met any of the following criteria: (1) refused to participate; (2) were deaf, dumb, blind, or had any other physical disability that may hinder completion of the survey; or (3) had any major psychiatric illness or history of mental disorders. A total of 3,230 respondents were interviewed, of whom 3,061 (94.77%) were provided valid responses. Ethical review of this study was approved by the Committee of the School of Public Health, Xiamen University. Informed consent was obtained from each participant on the first page of the questionnaire.

Measures

Cognitive function was measured by a brief and widely used cognitive screening tool – the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), which has high sensitivity and specificity for detecting mild cognitive impairment (Nasreddine et al., Reference Nasreddine2005). In this study, we used the MoCA-BJ, a Chinese version of the MoCA tailored to the local context, which has been reported to be a reliable and stable cognitive screening tool among Chinese older adults (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Zhang, Xiao, Dong, Niu and Kong2015). The MoCA-BJ assessed six cognitive domains: (1) the short-term memory recall task (five points) involved two learning trials of five nouns and delayed recall after approximately 5 minutes; (2) visuospatial abilities (four points) were assessed using the Clock Drawing Test (three points) and Copy of the Cube (one point); (3) multiple aspects of executive function (four points) were assessed using the Modified Trail Making Test (one point), Animals Fluency (one point), and a two-item verbal abstraction task (one point each); (4) attention (six points) was evaluated using a sustained attention task (Number 1 Tapping Test; 1 point), Serial 7 Subtractions (three points), Digit Span Forward (one point), and Digit span backward (one point); (5) language (six points) involved a three-item confrontation naming task with low-familiarity animals (lion, camel, and rhinoceros; three points), Sentence Repetition (two points), and the aforementioned fluency task (one point); and (6) orientation (six points) to time and place was evaluated (Nasreddine et al., Reference Nasreddine2005). Higher scores indicate better cognitive functions. The details of the scale, including the contents and scoring rules, can be found at http://www.mocatest.org/. Of note, to reduce the educational bias of the MoCA, two points were added for old adults with ≤6 years of education, while one point was added for those with 7~12 years of education (the points were not added if the total score exceeded 30 points after the addition) (Tan et al., Reference Tan2015). Furthermore, participants whose original MoCA scores were less than cut-off points for dementia were excluded, these cut-off points were 11, 14, and 16 for ≤5, 6~8, and ≥9 years of education, respectively (Yin et al., Reference Yin2014). In the end, 2,617 participants were included in the following analyses. Since no valid local studies to date have suggested the educational adjustments for MoCA sub-domains, we then further divided our eligible respondents into three groups by their education level (≤5, 6~8, and ≥9 years of education).

Among the exposure factors of interest (lifestyle factors), participants were asked to choose the best option that represented their past experience of such behaviors. For substance abuse, there were three options for cigarette smoking and alcohol drinking: “never,” “smoking/drinking now,” and “have quit now.” For leisure activity, continuous numeric values were used for TV watching and reading, which were assessed by the questions “How many hours did you spend watching TV per day last month on average?” and “How many hours did you spend reading books or newspapers per week last month on average?,” respectively. Smartphone use and social activity were measured by answers to the questions, “Did you frequently use a smartphone last month?” and “Did you frequently engage in social activity last month?,” respectively, with two alternative options of “yes” or “no.” Exercise was assessed by asking “How many days on average did you exercise in one week?” with five options: “never,” “1~2 days,” “3~4 days,” “5~6 days,” and “every day.”

Additionally, some demographic characteristics (age, gender, area, education, and marital status) and some medical and health factors (body mass index (BMI), hypertension, diabetes, and depression) were considered as covariates. Among them, BMI was classified into four categories based on recommendations by the Working Group On Obesity in China of the Chinese Ministry of Health (Zhou, Reference Zhou2002): <18.5 kg/m2 for underweight, 18.5~23.9 kg/m2 for normal weight, 24~27.9 kg/m2 for overweight, and ≥28 kg/m2 for obese. Chronic diseases were assessed by asking “Do you suffer from the following physician-diagnosed chronic diseases (check all that apply)?” Hypertension and diabetes were two options among the listed chronic diseases. Finally, the 15-item Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS-15) was used, and those who scored more than 4 would then be deemed to have depression (Kurlowicz and Greenberg, Reference Kurlowicz and Greenberg2007).

Data analysis

First, we summarized the characteristics of participants by using descriptive statistics (proportion, mean, and standard deviations) for each cognitive domain and compared the mean difference of scores by using analysis of variance (ANOVA). Serial proportional odds models were used separately in three education-specific groups – MoCA scores in each cognitive domain as the ordinal dependent variable and lifestyle factors and other covariates as independent variables – to estimate the associations between lifestyle factors and specific cognitive functions. Taking the scores in executive ability as the ordinal dependent variable as an example, the proportional odds model estimates four logits (log odds) as follows:

The model estimated separate intercepts for each logit (α1, α2, α3, and α4), but the proportional odds assumption limited the relationship between each explanatory variable and the dependent variable to be constant; that is, β i in each logit was the same. Similarly, a batch of proportional odds models estimated 5, 4, 6, 6, and 6 logits when we used the MoCA scores in memory, visuospatial abilities, attention, language, and orientation as dependent variables, respectively. We also used similar models to test the relationship between lifestyle factors and general cognition by using MoCA-adjusted total score percentiles (P25, P50, and P75) as the ordinal dependent variable. Socio-demographic factors were controlled in all models aforementioned. The models were fit with maximum likelihood estimation using SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary NC, 2017).

Results

Descriptive analyses

Among the 2,617 eligible participants, the overall mean age was 69.06 ± 7.06 years, with ages ranging from 60 to 96 years. Additionally, 54.03% of the participants were men and 54.41% were from urban areas. Nearly two-thirds attained less than nine years of education and 25.34% were unmarried. Regarding lifestyle factors, current smokers and drinkers accounted for 30.89% and 16.50%, respectively. Most of the respondents (94.72%) watched TV, but comparatively few participants (41.56%) read books or newspapers. Smartphone users accounted for over a quarter. Over half of the participants did not take part in social activity frequently, while one-third did not exercise for a single day per week or were overweight or obese. Participants suffering from hypertension, diabetes, or depression comprised 35.04%, 10.59%, and 9.81% of the sample, respectively. More detailed descriptive statistics are displayed in Table 1.

Table 1. Characteristics of 2,617 participants according to the MoCA scores and ANOVA results

Note: MoCA Score* has been adjusted according to the education year. Significant p-values (p < 0.05) are marked in bold.

Overall, the education-adjusted mean MoCA score was 21.97 ± 4.61. Participants who attained higher scores in general cognition and each cognitive domain were more likely to be younger, male, married, living in urban areas, and more formally educated. The ANOVA indicated that the mean scores of each cognitive domain were significantly different in almost all groups with lifestyle factors. Higher scores were more likely for ex-smokers or current drinkers. Participants who watched TV, read books/newspapers, used smartphones, exercised, or engaged in social activity scored higher than those who did not. Participants with hypertension or diabetes had similar scores as those without, while elderly individuals with depression showed significantly lower scores than those without depression in all six cognitive domains.

Odds ratios of lifestyle factors for better cognitive function

A series of proportional odds models predicting better cognitive function were fitted for the general MoCA (Table 2) and six sub-domains of MoCA (Figure 1) in the three education-specific groups. For each model, a χ 2 score test supported the appropriateness of the proportional odds model.

Table 2. The odds ratios (95% confidential interval) of lifestyle characteristics for higher MoCA scores in three education-specific groups

Note: Background factors (age, gender, area, education, and marital status) and medical and health factors (BMI, hypertension, diabetes, and depression) were controlled in all models. Significant p-values (p < 0.05) are marked in bold.

Figure 1. Odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals for better cognitive functions in six domains among three education-specific groups. Proportional odds models were adjusted for background factors (age, gender, area, education, marital status), and medical and health factors (BMI, hypertension, diabetes, depression) in all models. Symbols represent the point estimates (ORs) while vertical bars around the symbols are the corresponding 95% CIs. Reference groups: Cigarette: never smoke; Alcohol: never drink; TV watching: 0.1~2h/d; Reading: 0.1~3.5h/w; Smartphone use: no; Social activity: no; Exercise: 0d/w.

As Table 2 shows, previous or current smoking and drinking was not associated with MoCA scores in the general population, but unexpectedly, ex-smokers in the low education group demonstrated better cognition (OR = 2.22) than never smokers. Reading, smartphone use (OR = 1.71), and engagement in social activity (OR = 1.27) all contributed to better cognition. TV watching and exercise were not associated with general cognition in the whole population; however, for the low education group, never watching TV was harmful (OR = 0.48), and for the high educated group, exercising 5~7 days per week was beneficial (OR = 1.48).

Figure 1 presents the odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals for better cognitive function in the six domains among the three education-specific groups. Compared with never smokers, ex-smokers, and current smokers performed better in attention (OR(95%CI): 2.05(1.02 – 4.14)) and language (OR(95%CI): 1.78 (1.18 – 2.67)), respectively, in the low education group, while in the high education group, current smokers performed worse in visuospatial function (OR(95%CI): 0.60 (0.39 – 0.91)). Drinking showed no association with any sub-domain of cognition in all three education-specific groups. For old adults in the low education group, compared to watching TV less than 2 hours, never watching was detrimental to executive ability (OR(95%CI): 0.44(0.24 – 0.79)) and language (OR(95%CI): 0.27(0.15 – 0.47)), and longer watching times were beneficial to attention (2.1~4 hours/day: OR(95%CI): 1.35(1.02 – 1.79)) and orientation (2.1~4 hours/day: OR(95%CI): 1.47 (1.03 – 2.08); >4 hours/day: OR(95%CI): 1.63(1.02 – 2.6)). However, excessive TV watching (>4 hours/day) was inversely associated with visuospatial function (OR(95%CI): 0.49 (0.29 – 0.8)) for the moderately educated elderly, even though modest TV watching (2~4 hours/day) was conversely related to visuospatial function (OR(95%CI): 0.72(0.52 – 0.99)) and executive ability (OR(95%CI): 0.71(0.52 – 0.97)) for the elderly with a high education level. Similarly, compared to reading no more than 3.5 hours per week, never reading was harmful to executive ability and language in all three education-specific groups, and for the low education group in particular, never reading showed an inverse association with multi-domain cognitive functions involving visuospatial ability (OR(95%CI): 0.36(0.23 – 0.56)), executive ability (OR(95%CI): 0.34(0.21 – 0.54)), attention (OR(95%CI): 0.57(0.36 – 0.89)), language (OR(95%CI): 0.32 (0.21 – 0.51)), and orientation (OR(95%CI): 0.40 (0.2 – 0.78)). However, comparatively longer reading (>3.5 hours/week) was detrimental to memory (OR(95%CI): 0.59(0.42 – 0.82)) and language (OR(95%CI): 0.54(0.38 – 0.77)) in the high education group. Smartphone users had the edge over non-smartphone users in five cognitive domains for the high education group, including visuospatial ability (OR(95%CI): 1.54(1.14 – 2.1)), executive ability (OR(95%CI): 2.25(1.67 – 3.03)), attention (OR(95%CI): 1.71(1.18 – 2.46)), language (OR(95%CI): 1.80(1.31 – 2.47)), and orientation (OR(95%CI): 1.77(1.02 – 3.04)), while this edge was limited within comparatively fewer sub-domains of cognition for the low and moderate education groups. Those frequently engaged in social activity also performed better in multi-domain cognitive functions for all the groups, though this beneficial effect was significant only for executive ability (OR(95%CI): 1.88(1.29 – 2.75)) within old adults in the moderate education group and for memory (OR(95%CI): 1.47(1.09 – 1.97)) within the high education group. Likewise, those who exercised more than five days a week showed superior results to those who did not exercise for a single day per week in almost all the sub-domains of cognition for all the groups, but the findings were significantly better only in memory (OR(95%CI): 1.49(1.05 – 2.12)) and language (OR(95%CI): 1.53(1.06 – 2.21)) for old adults in the high education group.

Discussion

Based on our large-scale survey data, we tried to detect the associations of modifiable lifestyle factors with multi-domain cognitive functions in three education-specific groups. Leisure activity was associated with multi-domain cognition, while substance abuse showed only a limited association. It is worth noting that for the elderly with a low education level, leisure activities such as TV watching, reading, and smartphone use effectively contributed to better cognition, while reading and exercise were beneficial to cognition for old adults with a moderate and high education, respectively.

Substance abuse, on the whole, showed a limited association with cognition

The substances considered in this study were cigarettes and alcohol. Our study indicated that smoking was not associated with cognition, but previous smoking showed a positive relationship among elderly participants in the low education group; more specifically, previous and current smoking might be beneficial to attention and language, respectively, for people with a moderate or low education level, while current smoking might be detrimental to visuospatial functions for old adults with a high education level. Previous experimental findings also found that smoking could enhance attention and that this enhancement may be induced by nicotine (Amitai and Markou, Reference Amitai and Markou2008; Rezvani et al., Reference Rezvani, Kholdebarin, Dawson and Levin2008). Furthermore, Daniela and colleagues found that smokers performed better in verbal memory and language fluency than non-smokers (Caldirola et al., Reference Caldirola2013), and this result might be due to social smoking (Schane et al., Reference Schane, Glantz and Ling2009), hence stimulating language ability. However, thus far, studies about the association between cigarettes and cognition have not reached a consensus. A cohort study in Britain assessed the association between smoking and cognition by gender and found that male smokers, compared to never smokers, experienced faster cognitive decline in global cognition and executive function, whereas no difference appeared in women as smoking status varied (Sabia et al., Reference Sabia2012). Considering the controversy over its benefits for cognition and the confirmation of its detrimental effect on pulmonary diseases and cardiovascular diseases, we call on the public not to smoke.

Drinking showed no association with general cognition or any sub-domain of cognition in our study. Drinking has been found to be similarly irrelevant to general cognition, verbal memory, semantic fluency, and attention in a cohort study, but the heaviest drinkers had the lowest phonemic fluency scores (Gross et al., Reference Gross2011). However, Tiia's findings suggested frequent and infrequent drinkers had better executive function and episodic memory than never drinkers (Ngandu, Reference Ngandu2006). Dose-effect and beverage type might, to some extent, elucidate this discrepancy. A growing body of reports showed a U-shaped relationship, i.e. a beneficial effect on cognition related to moderate alcohol drinking (Reas et al., Reference Reas, Laughlin, Kritz-Silverstein, Barrett-Connor and McEvoy2016; Shimizu et al., Reference Shimizu, Oki, Mitani, Tsuchiya and Nabeshima2016). Moreover, several studies showed that only wine had a protective effect on cognition (Gu et al., Reference Gu2014). In this study, however, we did not study the amount of alcohol consumed or the type of drink. We recommend that future studies explore the association of alcohol consumption and types of alcohol with domain-specific cognitive function.

Leisure activity associated with multi-domain cognitive functions

For the elderly in the low education group, TV watching, smartphone use, and reading enhanced multiple domains of cognition. It was reported that TV watching, smartphone use, and reading generally could be regarded as cognitive activities, which are good for cognition (Doi et al., Reference Doi2013). Specifically, TV can serve as a source of media that equips the elderly with the latest information (Ostlund, Reference Ostlund2010), which might help them achieve better temporal orientation. Furthermore, the information on TV attracts viewers largely with sounds and colorful images (Sigman, Reference Sigman2007), and this, to some extent, might enhance their attention. Additionally, watching TV might increase the communication between TV viewers and others, as they can share and discuss conversation topics from TV watching (Sun et al., Reference Sun, Zhang and Fan2016). Similar benefits from reading might be akin to TV watching, as mentioned above. In addition, smartphone users generally have to be familiar with the spatial layout of the number pad and various functional buttons. Additionally, they might master how to use some complicated applications such as the camera, video, and social media (Ballagas et al., Reference Ballagas, Borchers, Rohs and Sheridan2006), and all of this therefore could serve as a form of cognitive training that enhances multi-domain cognitive ability.

A less sedentary lifestyle was beneficial for the highly educated old adults. Moderate (2.1~4 hours/day) or excessive (>4 hours/day) TV watching (though not significantly) and comparatively longer reading (>3.5 hours/week) in this study showed negative associations with cognition, while adequate exercise (5~7 days/week) showed a positive relationship in the highly educated old adults. A previous study also found that excessive TV watching might negatively affect cognition (Hoang et al., Reference Hoang2016) because too much time spent watching TV generally meant excessive sedentary behavior and less physical activity, which was not good for cognition (Falck et al., Reference Falck, Davis and Liu-Ambrose2016). Similar explanations were also applicable to reading. A cohort study conducted with older Mexican Americans also indicated that greater exercise was associated with reduced memory function decline (Ottenbacher et al., Reference Ottenbacher2014) and better language (Salinas et al., Reference Salinas, Messias, Morales-Campos and Parra-Medina2014).

Memory with comparatively less positive factors

Among the six sub-domains, the most affected by age was memory (Riddle, Reference Riddle2007). It should be noted that fewer factors observed in the current study were significantly associated with memory than with attention. In addition, those cognitive enhancement factors affecting memory, including social activity and exercise, worked only for the high education group. For one, this might be because memory was a complex process involving encoding, storage, and retrieval. In addition, the information gained from TV watching or reading was more likely to be at the input stage, which meant that the information input may not be encoded or stored well, let alone be retrieved. In contrast, regarding more social activity, such as staying in contact with friends or playing mahjong (in reality or by using a mobile app), as the output, this activity forced the elderly to use information or skills they had learned before, which required an intact memory process of encoding, storage, and retrieval. For another, by using molecular systems associated with synaptic plasticity and energy metabolism, exercise can activate the neural circuitry, which is important for learning (Gomez-Pinilla and Hillman, Reference Gomez-Pinilla and Hillman2013). Given that memory decline was one of the most common complaints among older adults, we appeal to the public to live an active lifestyle and especially to engage in more social activity and exercise.

Of note, our ANOVA results indicated that participants with hypertension or diabetes had similar MoCA scores as those without. To date, studies of hypertension and diabetes have not indicated a consistent relationship with cognition. A meta-analysis even found that hypertension treatments may reduce the risk of cognitive decline (Rouch et al., Reference Rouch2015). In addition, a recent study indicated that patients with diabetes might have higher odds of having vascular dementia but lower odds of having Alzheimer's dementia (Sherzai et al., Reference Sherzai2016).

Limitations

This study has a number of strengths. One of the main strengths is that we have taken into account a broad range of cognitive functions that had not been studied simultaneously before. Many previous studies focused mainly on general cognition or a fraction of cognitive domains. Moreover, we explored the associations between domain-specific cognitive function and modifiable lifestyle factors in education-specific groups, which offered a new perspective for specific individual interventions.

Nevertheless, some limitations should also be acknowledged. First, detailed information on the independent variables (for example, the type of drinking beverage and drinking frequency) was not collected, which impeded a deeper understanding of their effects on cognition. Second, domain-specific cognitive functions were assessed by sub-scores of the MoCA, which has been reported to be subject to educational bias. However, to reduce such bias, we divided our respondents into three groups according to participants’ education level, which was recommended by a similar local study – that study used the MoCA in a large sample of community-dwelling old adults whose dialects were also different from Mandarin to detect dementia and mild cognitive impairment and recommended optimal cut-offs stratified by education level.

Conclusion

This study contributes to the literature as the first study to describe lifestyle factors in relation to multi-domain cognitive functions according to participants’ education levels. In conclusion, the present results suggested that for elderly with a low education level, leisure activities such as TV watching, smartphone use, and reading could enhance multiple domains of cognition; for old adults with a high education level, a less sedentary lifestyle involving engaging in more exercise, avoiding excessive TV watching and reading was beneficial; and for the moderately educated elderly, reading is a good way to improve general cognition, executive function, and language. Thus, individual intervention strategies should be designed according to specific educational groups and sub-domains. More detailed information on the effect of lifestyle on specific cognitive domains and longitudinal analyses will be needed in the future.

Conflict of interest

None.

Description of authors’ roles

Manqiong Yuan and Jia Chen participated equally in study conception, design, and analysis, interpretation of results, and drafting of the paper. Yaofeng Han, Xingliang Wei, Zirong Ye, and Liangwen Zhang contributed significant time and labor to the survey. Y. Alicia Hong assisted with interpretation of results and made critical revisions to the paper for important intellectual content. Ya Fang supervised the study, participated in study conception and design and interpretation of results, and made critical revisions to the paper for important intellectual content. All the authors approved the final version for submission.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (grant number 81573257 and 81602941) and the Natural Science Foundation of Fujian Province (grant number (2016J0101). We really appreciate the Xiamen Municipal Committee on Aging for supporting this study and the efforts the staff and volunteers made by working on ageing in these communities. We are also grateful to the students from the School of Public Health, Xiamen University, who participated in the data entry and checking of returned surveys.

Footnotes

#

These authors contributed equally to this work.

References

Amitai, N. and Markou, A. (2008). Chronic nicotine improves cognitive performance in a test of attention but does not attenuate cognitive disruption induced by repeated phencyclidine administration. Psychopharmacology, 202, 275.Google Scholar
Ballagas, R., Borchers, J., Rohs, M. and Sheridan, J. G. (2006). The smart phone: a ubiquitous input device. IEEE Pervasive Computing, 5, 7077.Google Scholar
Baumgart, M., Snyder, H. M., Carrillo, M. C., Fazio, S., Kim, H. and Johns, H. (2015). Summary of the evidence on modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline and dementia: a population-based perspective. Alzheimers Dement, 11, 718726.Google Scholar
Bell-McGinty, S., Podell, K., Franzen, M., Baird, A. D. and Williams, M. J. (2002). Standard measures of executive function in predicting instrumental activities of daily living in older adults. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 17, 828834.Google Scholar
Caldirola, D. et al. (2013). Effects of cigarette smoking on neuropsychological performance in mood disorders: a comparison between smoking and nonsmoking inpatients. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 74, e130e136.Google Scholar
Callahan, C. M. (2017). Alzheimer's disease: individuals, dyads, communities, and costs. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 65, 892895.Google Scholar
Chen, X., Zhang, R., Xiao, Y., Dong, J., Niu, X. and Kong, W. (2015). Reliability and validity of the Beijing version of the montreal cognitive assessment in the evaluation of cognitive function of adult patients with OSAHS. PLoS One, 10, e0132361.Google Scholar
Czaja, S. J. et al. (2013). Factors influencing use of an e-health website in a community sample of older adults. Journal of the American Medical Informatics Association, 20, 277284.Google Scholar
Doi, T. et al. (2013). Cognitive activities and instrumental activity of daily living in older adults with mild cognitive impairment. Dementia & Geriatric Cognitive Disorders Extra, 3, 398406.Google Scholar
Falck, R. S., Davis, J. C. and Liu-Ambrose, T. (2016). What is the association between sedentary behaviour and cognitive function? A systematic review. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 51, 800811.Google Scholar
Gomez-Pinilla, F. and Hillman, C. (2013). The influence of exercise on cognitive abilities. Comprehensive Physiology, 3, 403428.Google Scholar
Gross, A. L. et al. (2011). Alcohol consumption and domain-specific cognitive function in older adults: longitudinal data from the Johns Hopkins precursors study. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 66, 3947.Google Scholar
Gu, Y. et al. (2014). Alcohol intake and brain structure in a multiethnic elderly cohort. Clinical Nutrition, 33, 662667.Google Scholar
Hoang, T. D. et al. (2016). Effect of early adult patterns of physical activity and television viewing on midlife cognitive function. JAMA Psychiatry, 73, 7379.Google Scholar
Insel, K., Morrow, D., Brewer, B. and Figueredo, A. (2006). Executive function, working memory, and medication adherence among older adults. Journals of Gerontology Series B-Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 61, P102P107.Google Scholar
IOM (2015). Cognitive Aging: Progress in Understanding and Opportunities for Action. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.Google Scholar
Kurlowicz, L. and Greenberg, S. A. (2007). The geriatric depression scale (GDS). AJN The American Journal of Nursing, 107, 6768.Google Scholar
Lopes, M. A., Ferrioli, E., Nakano, E. Y., Litvoc, J. and Bottino, C. M. (2012). High prevalence of dementia in a community-based survey of older people from Brazil: association with intellectual activity rather than education. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 32, 307316.Google Scholar
Nasreddine, Z. S. et al. (2005). The montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 53, 695699.Google Scholar
Ng, T. P., Lim, M. L., Niti, M. and Collinson, S. (2012). Long-term digital mobile phone use and cognitive decline in the elderly. Bioelectromagnetics, 33, 176185.Google Scholar
Ngandu, T. (2006). Lifestyle risk factors in dementia and mild cognitive impairment: a population-based study. Department of Neurobiology Karolinska University Press: Karolinska Institutet.Google Scholar
Ohara, T. et al. (2015). Midlife and late-life smoking and risk of dementia in the community: the Hisayama study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 63, 23322339.Google Scholar
Ormstad, H., Rosness, T. A., Bergem, A. L., Bjertness, E., Strand, B. H. and Group, G. (2016). Alcohol consumption in the elderly and risk of dementia related death–a Norwegian prospective study with a 17-year follow-up. International Journal of Neuroscience, 126, 135144.Google Scholar
Ostlund, B. (2010). Watching television in later life: a deeper understanding of TV viewing in the homes of old people and in geriatric care contexts. Scandinavian Journal of Caring Sciences, 24, 233243.Google Scholar
Ottenbacher, A. J. et al. (2014). Role of physical activity in reducing cognitive decline in older Mexican–American adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 62, 17861791.Google Scholar
Reas, E. T., Laughlin, G. A., Kritz-Silverstein, D., Barrett-Connor, E. and McEvoy, L. K. (2016). Moderate, regular alcohol consumption is associated with higher cognitive function in older community-dwelling adults. The Journal of Prevention of Alzheimer's Disease, 3, 105113.Google Scholar
Rezvani, A. H., Kholdebarin, E., Dawson, E. and Levin, E. D. (2008). Nicotine and clozapine effects on attentional performance impaired by the NMDA antagonist dizocilpine in female rats. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 11, 6370.Google Scholar
Riddle, D. R. (2007). Brain Aging: Models, Methods, and Mechanisms. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.Google Scholar
Rouch, L. et al. (2015). Antihypertensive drugs, prevention of cognitive decline and dementia: a systematic review of observational studies, randomized controlled trials and meta-analyses, with discussion of potential mechanisms. CNS Drugs, 29, 113130.Google Scholar
Sabia, S. et al. (2012). Impact of smoking on cognitive decline in early old age: the Whitehall II cohort study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69, 627635.Google Scholar
Salinas, J. J., Messias, D. K. H., Morales-Campos, D. and Parra-Medina, D. (2014). English language proficiency and physical activity among Mexican-Origin women in South Texas and South Carolina. Journal of Health Care for the Poor and Underserved, 25, 357375.Google Scholar
Samanez-Larkin, G. R., Levens, S. M., Perry, L. M., Dougherty, R. F. and Knutson, B. (2012). Frontostriatal white matter integrity mediates adult age differences in probabilistic reward learning. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 53335337.Google Scholar
Schane, R. E., Glantz, S. A. and Ling, P. M. (2009). Social smoking implications for public health, clinical practice, and intervention research. American journal of preventive medicine, 37, 124131.Google Scholar
Sherzai, D. et al. (2016). The association between diabetes and dementia among elderly individuals: a nationwide inpatient sample analysis. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 29, 120125.Google Scholar
Shimizu, C., Oki, Y., Mitani, Y., Tsuchiya, Y. and Nabeshima, T. (2016). Moderate-dose regular lifelong alcohol intake changes the intestinal flora, protects against aging, and keeps spatial memory in the senescence-accelerated mouse prone 8 (SAMP8) model. Journal of Pharmaceutical Sciences, 19, 430447.Google Scholar
Shin, J. S. et al. (2013). Excessive TV watching in patients with frontotemporal dementia. Neurocase, 19, 489496.Google Scholar
Sigman, A. (2007). Visual voodoo: the biological impact of watching TV. Biologist, 54, 1217.Google Scholar
Sun, S. J., Zhang, S. Y. and Fan, X. T. (2016). Media use, cognitive performance, and life satisfaction of the Chinese elderly. Health Communication, 31, 12231234.Google Scholar
Tan, J. P. et al. (2015). Optimal cutoff scores for dementia and mild cognitive impairment of the montreal cognitive assessment among elderly and oldest-old Chinese population. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 43, 14031412.Google Scholar
Wong, A. et al. (2016). Relations between recent past leisure activities with risks of dementia and cognitive functions after stroke. PLoS One, 11, e0159952.Google Scholar
Yin, S. et al. (2014). The application of mini-mental state examination and montreal cognitive assessment for mild cognitive impairment and dementia in community survey. Chinese Journal of Psychiatry, 47, 293297.Google Scholar
Zhou, B. F. (2002). Predictive values of body mass index and waist circumference for risk factors of certain related diseases in Chinese adults–study on optimal cut-off points of body mass index and waist circumference in Chinese adults. Biomedical and Environmental Sciences, 15, 8396.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Characteristics of 2,617 participants according to the MoCA scores and ANOVA results

Figure 1

Table 2. The odds ratios (95% confidential interval) of lifestyle characteristics for higher MoCA scores in three education-specific groups

Figure 2

Figure 1. Odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals for better cognitive functions in six domains among three education-specific groups. Proportional odds models were adjusted for background factors (age, gender, area, education, marital status), and medical and health factors (BMI, hypertension, diabetes, depression) in all models. Symbols represent the point estimates (ORs) while vertical bars around the symbols are the corresponding 95% CIs. Reference groups: Cigarette: never smoke; Alcohol: never drink; TV watching: 0.1~2h/d; Reading: 0.1~3.5h/w; Smartphone use: no; Social activity: no; Exercise: 0d/w.