Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-m9kch Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-30T15:39:24.435Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Concept and Method in Cross-Cultural and Cultural Psychology

Conceptual and Methodological Issues in Cross-Cultural and Cultural Psychology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 October 2021

Ype H. Poortinga
Affiliation:
Tilburg University, The Netherlands

Summary

An overview is given of cross-cultural psychology and cultural psychology, focusing on theory and methodology. In Section 1 historical developments in research are traced; it is found that initially extensive psychological differences tend to shrink when more carefully designed studies are conducted. Section 2 addresses the conceptualization of “culture” and of “a culture”. For psychological research the notion “culture” is considered too vague; more focal explanatory concepts are required. Section 3 describes methodological issues, taking the notion of the empirical cycle as a lead for both qualitative and quantitative research. Pitfalls in research design and data analysis of behavior-comparative studies, and the need for replication are discussed. Section 4 suggests to move beyond research on causal relationships and to incorporate additional questions, addressing the function and the development of behavior patterns in ontogenetic, phylogenetic and historical time. Section 5 emphasizes the need for applied research serving the global village.
Get access
Type
Element
Information
Online ISBN: 9781108908320
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication: 04 November 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adair, J., & Diaz-Loving, R. (1999). Indigenous psychologies: The meaning of the concept and its assessment. Applied Psychology, 48, 397402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, B. G., & van de Vijver, F. J. R. (2017). Identity and acculturation: The case for Africa. Journal of Psychology in Africa, 27, 115121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2013, May 3). Release Number 13–33.Google Scholar
Andrews, P. W., Gangestad, S. W., & Matthews, D. (2002). Adaptationism – how to carry out an exaptationist program. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 489553.Google Scholar
Aplin, L. M., Farine, D. R., Morand-Ferron, J., Cockburn, A., Thornton, A., & Sheldon, B. C. (2015). Experimentally induced innovations lead to persistent culture via conformity in wild birds. Nature, 518, 538541.Google Scholar
Arends-Tóth, J., & van de Vijver, F. (2003). Multiculturalism and acculturation: Views of Dutch and Turkish-Dutch. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 249266.Google Scholar
Ariès, P. (1960). L’enfant et la vie familiale sous l’Ancien Régime [Child and family life in the Old Order]. Paris: Éditions du Seuil.Google Scholar
Atran, S. (2007). Religion’s social and cognitive landscape. In Kitayama, S. & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (pp. 437453). New York: Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. R., Faulkner, S. L., & Hecht, M. L. (2006). A moving target: The illusive definition of culture. In Baldwin, J. R., Faulkner, S. L., Hecht, M. L., & Lindsley, S. L. (Eds.), Redefining culture: Perspectives across the disciplines (pp. 326). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Baltes, P. B. (1997). On the incomplete architecture of human ontogeny. American Psychologist, 52, 366380.Google Scholar
Baltes, P. B., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Rösler, F. (Eds.). (2006). Lifespan development and the brain: The perspective of biocultural co-constructivism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Barbarin, O. A., & Richter, L. M. (2001). Mandela’s children: Growing up in post-apartheid South Africa. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Barker, R. G., & Wright, L. S. (1951). One boy’s day: A specimen record of behavior. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., Adolphs, R., Marsella, S., Martinez, A. M., & Pollak, S. D. (2019). Emotional expressions reconsidered: Challenges to inferring emotion from human facial movements. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 20(1), 168.Google Scholar
Bebko, G. M., Cheon, B. K., Ochsner, K. N., & Chiao, J. Y. (2019). Cultural differences in perceptual strategies underlying emotion. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 50, 10141026.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J. (2012). The development of human reproductive strategies: Progress and prospects. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(5), 310316.Google Scholar
Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience, interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary theory of socialization. Child Development, 62, 647670.Google Scholar
Bender, M., & Adams, B. G. (Eds.). (2021). Methods and assessment in culture and psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Benítez, I., Padilla, J. L., van de Vijver, F., & Cuevas, A. (2018). What cognitive interviews tell us about bias in cross-cultural research: An illustration using quality-of-life items. Field Methods, 30(4), 277294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berlin, B., & Kay, P. (1969). Basic color terms: Their universality and evolution. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Bernardi, J., & Jobson, L. (2019). Investigating the moderating role of culture on the relationship between appraisals and symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder. Clinical Psychological Science, 7, 10001013.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W. (1980). Acculturation as varieties of adaptation. In Padilla, A. (Ed.), Acculturation: Theory, models and some new findings (pp. 925). Boulder, CO: Westview.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W. (1997). Immigration, acculturation and adaptation. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 46, 568.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W. (2001). A psychology of immigration. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 615631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berry, J. W., Phinney, J. S., Sam, D. L., & Vedder, P. (Eds.). (2006). Immigrant youth in cultural transition: Acculturation, identity, and adaptation across national contexts. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., Breugelmans, S. M., Chasiotis, A., & Sam, D. L. (2011). Cross-cultural psychology: Research and applications (3rd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bimler, D. (2007). From color naming to a language space: An analysis of data from the World Color Survey. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 7, 173199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birdwhistell, R. L. (1970). Kinesics and context. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Boehnke, K., Lietz, P., Schreier, M., & Wilhelm, A. (2011). Sampling: The selection of cases for culturally comparative psychological research. In Matsumoto, D. & Van de Vijver, F. J. R. (Eds.), Cross-cultural research methods in psychology (pp. 102129). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Boer, D., Hanke, K., & He, J. (2018). On detecting systematic measurement error in cross-cultural research: A review and critical reflection on equivalence and invariance tests. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 49, 713734.Google Scholar
Bolhuis, J. J., & Verhulst, S. (2009). Tinbergen’s legacy: Function and mechanism in behavioral biology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bolhuis, J. J., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2009). Can evolution explain how minds work? Nature, 458, 832833.Google Scholar
Boroditsky, L., Schmidt, L. A., & Phillips, W. (2003). In Gentner, D. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (Eds.), Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought (pp. 6179). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Botvinik-Nezer, R., Holzmeister, F., Camerer, C. F., Dreber, A., Huber, J. Johannesson, M.Scholnberg, T. (2020). Variability in the analysis of a single neuroimaging dataset by many teams. Nature, 582, 8488.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bourhis, R., Moise, C., Perreault, S., & Senecal, S. (1997). Towards an interactive acculturation model: A social psychological approach. International Journal of Psychology, 32, 369386.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss. Vol. I: Attachment. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Breugelmans, S. M., & Poortinga, Y. H. (2006). Emotion without a word: Shame and guilt with Rarámuri Indians and rural Javanese. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 11111122.Google Scholar
Brewer, M. B. (2007). The importance of being we: Human nature and intergroup relations. American Psychologist, 62, 728738.Google Scholar
Brewer, M., & Campbell, D. T. (1976). Ethnocentrism and intergroup attitudes: East African evidence. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Brouwers, S. A., Van Hemert, D. A., Breugelmans, S. M., & van de Vijver, F. J. R. (2004). A historical analysis of empirical studies published in the Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 1970–2004. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 251262.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bryman, A. (2015). Social research methods (5th ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 114.Google Scholar
Button, K. S., Ioannidis, J. P. A., Mokrysz, C., Nosek, J. F., Robinson, E. S. J., & Munafò, M. R. Nosek, J. (2013). Power failure: Why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14, 365376.Google Scholar
Byrne, B. M., & Van de Vijver, F. J. R. (2010). Testing for measurement and structural equivalence in large-scale cross-cultural studies: Addressing the issue of nonequivalence. International Journal of Testing, 10, 107132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1964). Distinguishing differences of perception from failures of communication in cross-cultural studies. In Northrop, F. S. C. & Livingston, H. H. (Eds.), Cross-cultural understanding: Epistemology in anthropology (pp. 308336). New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Campbell, D. T., & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 81105.Google Scholar
Carey, S. (2008). Précis of The origin of concepts. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34, 113167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. (1981). Cultural transmission and evolution: A quantitative approach. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Chen, C., & Moyzis, R. K. (2018). Cultural genomics: Promises and challenges. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 49, 764788.Google Scholar
Cheung, F. M., Leung, K., Fan, R. M., Song, W., Zhang, J., & Zhang, J. (1996). Development of the Chinese Personality Assessment Inventory. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 27, 181199.Google Scholar
Cheung, S. F., Cheung, F. M., Howard, R., & Lin, Y.-H. (2006). Personality across the ethnic divide in Singapore: Are “Chinese traits” uniquely Chinese? Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 467477.Google Scholar
Chiao, J. Y., Harada, T., Komeda, H., Li, A., Mano, Y., Saito, D.Iidaka, T. (2009). Neural basis of individualistic and collectivistic views of self. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 28132820.Google Scholar
Child, I. L. (1954). Socialization. In Lindzey, G. (Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 655692). Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Church, A. T. (2016). Personality traits across cultures. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 2230.Google Scholar
Cohen, D., & Kitayama, S. (Eds.) (2019). Handbook of cultural psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1994). The earth is round (p < .05). American Psychologist, 49, 9971003.Google Scholar
Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Cole, M., & Cole, S. R. (1996). The development of children (3rd ed.). New York Freeman.Google Scholar
Cole, M., Gay, J., Glick, J., & Sharp, D. (1971). The cultural context of learning and thinking. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Cole, M., & Packer, M. (2011). Culture in development. In Lamb, M. E. & Bornstein, M. H. (Eds.), Social and personality development: An advanced textbook (6th ed., pp. 51108). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Conroy-Beam, D., Roneya, J. R., Lukaszewski, A. W., Buss, D. M., Asao, K., Sorokowska, A. Zupančič, M. (2019). Assortative mating and the evolution of desirability covariation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 40, 479491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corbin, J. M., & Strauss, A. (1990). Grounded theory research: Procedures, canons, and evaluative criteria. Qualitative Sociology, 13(1), 321.Google Scholar
Creswell, J. W. (2009). Research design: Qualitative, quantitative, and mixed methods approaches (3rd ed.). Los Angeles: Sage.Google Scholar
Cronbach, L. J., Gleser, G. C., Nanda, H., & Rajaratnam, N. (1972). The dependability of behavioral measurements. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Cuthill, I. (2009). The study of function in behavioral ecology. In Bolhuis, J. J. & Verhulst, S. (Eds.) (2009). Tinbergen’s legacy: Function and mechanism in behavioral biology (pp. 107126). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dasen, P. R. (1972). Cross-cultural Piagetian research: A summary. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 7, 7585.Google Scholar
Dasen, P. R. (1984). The cross-cultural study of intelligence: Piaget and the Baoulé. International Journal of Psychology, 19, 407434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dasen, P. R., & Mishra, R. C. (2010). Development of geocentric spatial language and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
de Groot, A. D., & Spiekerman, J. A. A. (1969). Methodologies: Foundations of inference and research in the behavioral sciences. The Hague: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. (2008). Putting the altruism back into altruism: The evolution of empathy. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 279300.Google Scholar
Deręgowski, J. B. (1980). Illusions, patterns and pictures: A cross-cultural perspective. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Deręgowski, J. B. (1989). Real space and represented space: Cross-cultural perspectives. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 5174.Google Scholar
Diaz-Guerrero, R. (1975). Psychology of the Mexican: Culture and personality. Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Diener, E., Ng, W., Harter, J., & Arora, R. (2010). Wealth and happiness across the world: Material prosperity predicts life evaluation, whereas psychosocial prosperity predicts positive feeling. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 5261.Google Scholar
Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Lucas, R. E. (2015). National accounts of subjective well-being. American Psychologist, 70, 234242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dimitrova, R., Chasiotis, A., Bender, M., & Van de Vijver, F. J. R. (2014). Collective identity and well-being of Bulgarian Roma adolescents and their mothers. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 43, 375386.Google Scholar
Doan, S. N., & Wang, Q. (2018). Children’s emotion knowledge and internalizing problems: The moderating role of culture. Transcultural Psychiatry, 55, 689709.Google Scholar
Doyen, S., Klein, O., Pichon, C.-L., & Cleeremans, A. (2012). Behavioral priming: It’s all in the mind, but whose mind? PLoS ONE, 7 (1), ArtID: e29081.Google Scholar
Draguns, J. G., & Tanaka-Matsumi, J. (2003). Assessment of psychopathology across and within cultures: Issues and findings. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 755775.Google Scholar
Easterly, W. (2006). The white man’s burden: Why the West’s efforts to aid the rest have done so much ill and so little good. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17, 124129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elfenbein, H. A. (2013). Nonverbal dialects and accents in facial expressions of emotion. Emotion Review, 5, 9096.Google Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A metaanalysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 203235.Google Scholar
Ellemers, N., Fiske, S. T., Abele, A. E., Koch, A., & Yzerbyt, V. (2020). Adversarial alignment enables competing models to engage in cooperative theory building toward cumulative science. PNAS, 117(14), 75617567.Google Scholar
Evans, K., Rotello, C. M., Li, X. S., & Rayner, K. (2009). Scene perception and memory revealed by eye movements and receiver-operating characteristic analyses: Does a cultural difference truly exist? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 276285.Google Scholar
Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2003). The nature of human altruism. Nature, 425, 785791.Google Scholar
Fischer, R. (2018). Personality, values, culture: An evolutionary approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fischer, R. (2020). People overestimate cultural differences in human values: The paradox of essentializing culture. Manuscript in preparation.Google Scholar
Fischer, R., & Poortinga, Y. H. (2012). Are cultural values the same as the values of individuals? An examination of similarities in personal, social and cultural value structure. International Journal of Cross-Cultural Management, 12, 157170.Google Scholar
Fischer, R., & Schwartz, S. H. (2011). Whence differences in value priorities? Individual, cultural, or artifactual sources. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42, 11271144.Google Scholar
Fischer, R., Vauclair, C. M., Fontaine, J. J. R., & Schwartz, S. H. (2010). Are individual-level and country-level value structures different? Testing Hofstede’s legacy with the Schwartz Value Survey. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41, 135151.Google Scholar
Fontaine, J. (2005). Equivalence. In Kempf-Leonard, K. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of social measurement (Vol. 1, pp. 803813). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Fontaine, J. R. J., & Veirman, E., & Groenvynck, H. (2013). The dimensional structure of emotional experiences. In Fontaine, J. J. R., Scherer, K. R., & Soriano, C. (Eds.), Components of emotional meaning: A sourcebook (pp. 233242). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fortin Morales, A. M., Van de Vijver, F. J. R., & Poortinga, Y. H. (2013). Differential item functioning and educational risk factors in Guatemalan reading assessment. Revista Interamericana de Psicología, 47, 423432.Google Scholar
Fuentes, A. (2006). Evolution is important but it is not simple: Defining cultural traits and incorporating complex evolutionary theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 354355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geertz, C. (1973). The interpretation of cultures. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gelfand, M. J., Raver, J. L., Nishii, L., Leslie, L. M., Lun, J., Lim, B. C.Yamaguchi, S. (2011). Differences between tight and loose cultures: A 33-nation study. Science, 332, 11001104.Google Scholar
Georgas, J., Berry, J. W., Van de Vijver, F., Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç. , & Poortinga, Y. (Eds.) (2006). Families across cultures: A 30-nation psychological study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gergen, K. J. (1985). The social constructionist movement in modern psychology. American Psychologist, 40, 266275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gesell, A. (1940). The first five years of life: A guide to the study of the preschool child (Part I). New York: Harper.Google Scholar
Girndt, T., & Poortinga, Y. H. (1997). Interculturele communicatie: Conventies en misverstanden. [Intercultural communication: Conventions and misunderstandings] De Psycholoog, 32, 299304.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behavior. Boston: Belknap.Google Scholar
Goto, H. (1971). Auditory perception by normal Japanese adults of sounds of “l” or “r.Neuropsychologia, 9, 317323.Google Scholar
Gould, S. J., & Lewontin, R. C. (1979). The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: A critique of the adaptationist programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London (Series B), 205, 581598.Google Scholar
Gould, S. J., & Vrba, E. S. (1982). Exaptation: A missing term in the science of form. Paleobiology, 8, 415.Google Scholar
Grantham-McGregor, S., Cheung, Y. B., Cueto, S., Glewwe, P., Richter, L. Strupp, B. & International Child Development Steering Group. (2007). Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries. Lancet, 369, 6070.Google Scholar
Gross, P. R., Levitt, N., & Lewis, M. W. (Eds.) (1996). The flight from science and reason. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 775. New York: New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Guba, E. G. (1981). Criteria for assessing the trustworthiness of naturalistic inquiries. Educational Communication and Technology Journal, 29, 7591.Google Scholar
Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behavior, I, II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 152.Google Scholar
Han, S. H., & Northoff, G. (2008). Culture-sensitive neural substrates of human cognition: A transcultural neuroimaging approach. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 646654.Google Scholar
Hao, J., Li, D., Peng, L., Peng, S., & Torelli, C. J. (2016). Advancing our understanding of culture mixing. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 47, 12571267.Google Scholar
Haven, T. L., & Van Grootel, L. (2019). Preregistering qualitative research. Accountability in Research, 26, 229244.Google Scholar
He, J., & Van de Vijver, F. J. R., , Espinosa Domínguez, A., & Mui, P. H. C. (2014). Toward a unification of acquiescent, extreme, and midpoint response styles: A multilevel study. International Journal of Cross Cultural Management, 14, 306322.Google Scholar
Hedden, T., Ketay, S., Aron, A., Markus, H. R., & Gabriel, J. D. E. (2008). Cultural influences on neural substrates of attentional control. Psychological Science, 19, 1217.Google Scholar
Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Peng, K. P., & Greenholtz, J. (2002). What’s wrong with cross-cultural comparisons of subjective Likert scales? The reference-group effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 903918.Google Scholar
Helliwell, J. F., Layard, R., Sachs, J., & De Neve, J.-E. (Eds.). (2020). World Happiness Report 2020. New York: Sustainable Development Solutions Network.Google Scholar
Heron, A., & Kroeger, E. (1981). Introduction to developmental psychology. In Triandis, H. C. & Heron, A. (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 115). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Hespos, S. J., & Spelke, E. S. (2004). Conceptual precursors to language. Nature, 430, 453456.Google Scholar
Hewlett, B. S., Fouts, H. N., Boyette, A. H., & Hewlett, B. L. (2012). Social learning among Congo Basin hunter-gatherers. In Whiten, A., Hinde, R. A., Stringer, C. B., & Laland, K. N., (Eds.), Culture evolves (pp. 411430). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hewlett, B. S., & Lamb, M. E. (2002). Integrating evolution, culture and developmental psychology: Explaining caregiver-infant proximity and responsiveness in central Africa and the USA. In Keller, H., Poortinga, Y. H., & Schölmerich, A. (Eds.). (2002). Between culture and biology: Perspectives on ontogenetic development. (pp. 241269). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hilbe, C., Schmid, L., Tkadlec, J., Chatterjee, K., & Nowak, M. A. (2018). Indirect reciprocity with private, noisy, and incomplete information. PNAS, 115(48), 1224112246.Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A. (1987). Individuals, relationships, and culture: Links between ethology and the social sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hinton, D. E., Reis, R., & de Jong, J. (2020). Ghost encounters among traumatized Cambodian refugees: Severity, relationship to PTSD, and phenomenology. Culture, Medicine, and Psychiatry, 44(3), 333359.Google Scholar
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences: International differences in work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Hogg, M. A. (2014). From uncertainty to extremism: Social categorization and identity processes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 338342.Google Scholar
Hong, Y., Morris, M., Chiu, C., & Benet-Martínez, V. (2000). Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist, 55, 709720.Google Scholar
Honing, H., ten Cate, C., Peretz, I., & Trehub, S. E. (2015). Without it no music: Cognition, biology and evolution of musicality. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society (Series B) 370: 2014.0088.Google Scholar
Hopkins, B., & Westra, T. (1990). Motor development, maternal expectations, and the role of handling. Infant Behavior and Development, 13, 117122.Google Scholar
Hox, J. (2010). Multilevel analysis: Techniques and applications (2nd ed.). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Huang, Y., Tse, C. S., & Cho, K. W. (2014). Living in the North is not necessarily favorable: Different metaphoric associations between cardinal direction and valence in Hong Kong and in the United States. European Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 360369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iacoboni, M. (2009). Imitation, empathy, and mirror neurons. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 653670.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2005). Why most published research findings are false. PLoS Medicine 2(8), e124.Google Scholar
Jahoda, G. (1992). Crossroads between culture and mind. New York: Harvester Wheatsheaf.Google Scholar
Jahoda, G. (2011). Past and present of cross-cultural psychology. In Van de Vijver, F. J. R., Chasiotis, A., & Breugelmans, S. M. (Eds.), Fundamental questions in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 37–63). Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Jahoda, G. (2012). Critical reflections on some recent definitions of “culture.Culture & Psychology, 18(3), 289303.Google Scholar
Jahoda, G., & Krewer, B. (1997). History of cross-cultural and cultural psychology. In Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., & Pandey, J. (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology: Theory and method (2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 142). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Kağıtçıbaşı, C. (1997). Individualism and collectivism. In Berry, J. W., Segall, M. H., & Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology (2nd ed., Vol 3, pp. 149). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Karasz, A. (2011). Qualitative and mixed methods research in cross-cultural psychology. In Van de Vijver, F. J. R., Chasiotis, A., & Breugelmans, S. M. (Eds.), Fundamental questions in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 214234). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kashima, Y., & Gelfand, M. J. (2012). A history of culture in psychology. In Kruglanski, A. W. & Stroebe, W. (Eds.), Handbook of the history of social psychology (pp. 499520). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Kay, P., Berlin, B., Maffi, L., Merrifield, W. R., & Cook, R. (2009). The World Color Survey. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Keller, H. (2002). Development as the interface between biology and culture: A conceptualization of early ontogenetic experiences. In Keller, H., Poortinga, Y., & Schoelmerich, A. (Eds.), Between culture and biology (pp. 215240). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, H. (2019). Culture and development. In Kitayama, S. & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (2nd ed., pp. 397423). New York: Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Keller, H., Bard, K., Morelli, G. Chaudhary, N., Vicedo, M., Rosabal‐Coto, M.Gottlieb, A. (2018). The myth of universal sensitive responsiveness: Comment on Mesman et al. (2017). Child Development, 89, 19211928.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, H., Poortinga, Y. H., & Schoelmerich, A. (Eds.). (2002). Between culture and biology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kim, U., Yang, K.-S., & Hwang, K.-K. (Eds.). (2006). Indigenous and cultural psychology: Understanding people in context. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., & Cohen, D. (Eds.). (2007). Handbook of cultural psychology. New York: Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., & Markus, H. R. (1994). Introduction to cultural psychology and emotion research. In Kitayama, S. & Markus, H. R. (Eds.), Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual influence (pp. 119). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Klein, R. A., Ratliff, K. A., Vianello, M., Adams, R. B., Bahnik, Š., Bernstein, M. J. … Nosek, B. A. (2014). Investigating variation in replicability: A “Many Labs” replication project. Social Psychology, 45, 142152.Google Scholar
Klein, R. A., Vianello, M., Hasselman, F., Adams, B. G., Adams, R. B., Aler, S. … Nosek, B. A. (2018). Many Labs 2: Investigating variation in replicability across samples and settings. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1, 443490.Google Scholar
Kroeber, A. L., & Kluckhohn, C. (1952). Culture: A critical review of concepts and definitions. Vol. 47, no. 1. Cambridge, MA: Peabody Museum.Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kuper, A., & Marks, J. (2011). Anthropologists unite! Nature, 470, 166168.Google Scholar
Lakatos, I. (1974). Falsification and the methodology of scientific research programmes. In Lakatos, I. & Musgrave, A. (Eds.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge (pp. 91196). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, J., & Feldman, M. W. (2000). Niche construction, biological evolution, and cultural change. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 131146.Google Scholar
Laland, K. N., Uller, T., Feldman, M. W. Sterelny, K., Müller, G. B., Moczek, A. … Odling-Smee J. (2015). The extended evolutionary synthesis: Its structure, assumptions and predictions. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282 (1813): 2015.1019.Google Scholar
Lamb, M. E., & Lewis, C. (2011). The role of parent-child relationships in child development. In Lamb, M. E. & Bornstein, M. H. (Eds.), Social and personality development: An advanced textbook (6th ed., pp. 469518). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Lee, J. J., Wedow, R., Okbay, A., Kong, E. M., Maghzian, O., Zacher, M.Cesarini-Show, D. (2018). Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a genome-wide association study of educational attainment in 1.1 million individuals. Nature Genetics, 50, 11121121.Google Scholar
Lerner, R. M., Lewin-Bizan, S., & Warren, A. E. A. (2011). Concepts and theories of human development. In Bornstein, M. H. & Lamb, M. H. (Eds.). (2011). Cognitive development: An advanced textbook (pp. 1965). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Leung, K., & Bond, M. H. (2004). Social axioms: A model for social beliefs in multicultural perspective. In Zanna, M. P. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 36, pp. 119197). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. (2003). Space in language and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lewis, O. (1966). La vida. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Liebal, K., & Haun, D. B. M. (2018). Why cross-cultural psychology is incomplete without comparative and developmental perspectives. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 49, 751763.Google Scholar
Lindsey, D. T., & Brown, A. M. (2009). World Color Survey color naming reveals universal motifs and their within-language diversity. PNAS, 106, 47, 1978519790.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucas, R., & Diener, E. (2008). Can we learn about national differences in happiness from individual responses? A multilevel approach. In Van de Vijver, F. J. R., Van Hemert, D. A., & Poortinga, Y. H. (Eds.), Individuals and cultures in multilevel analysis (pp. 223248). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lumsden, C. J., & Wilson, E. O. (1981). Genes, mind and culture: The coevolutionary process. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Luria, A. K. (1971). Towards the problem of the historical nature of psychological processes. International Journal of Psychology, 6, 259272.Google Scholar
Lutz, C. A. (1988). Unnatural emotions: Everyday sentiments on a Micronesian atoll and their challenge to Western theory. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lykken, D. T. (1968). Statistical significance in psychological research. Psychological Bulletin, 70(3), 151-159.Google Scholar
Lyman, R. L. (2006). Cultural traits and cultural integration. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 357358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Majid, A., Bowerman, M., Kita, S., Haun, D. B. M., & Levinson, S. C. (2004). Can language restructure cognition? The case for space. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 108114.Google Scholar
Malpass, R. S., & Poortinga, Y. H. (1986). Strategies for design and analysis. In Lonner, W. J. & Berry, J. W. (Eds.), Field methods in cross-cultural research (pp. 4784). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Manning, A., & Dawkins, M. S. (2012). An introduction to animal behavior. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 244253.Google Scholar
Maseland, R., & Van Hoorn, A. (2009). Explaining the negative correlation between values and practices: A note on the Hofstede-GLOBE debate. Journal of International Business Studies, 40, 527532.Google Scholar
Masuda, T., & Nisbett, R. E. (2001). Attending holistically versus analytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 922934.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matsumoto, D. (1999). Culture and self: An empirical assessment of Markus and Kitayama’s theory of independent and interdependent self-construals. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 2, 289310.Google Scholar
Matsumoto, D., & Willingham, B. (2006). The thrill of victory and the agony of defeat: Spontaneous expressions of medal winners at the 2004 Athens Olympic Games. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 568581.Google Scholar
Maynard Smith, J., & Harper, D. (2003). Animal signals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mayr, E. (1961). Cause and effect in biology. Science, 134, 15011506.Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R., & Terracciano, A. (2008). The five-factor model and its correlates in individuals and cultures. In Van de Vijver, F. J. R., Van Hemert, D. A., & Poortinga, Y. H. (Eds.), Multilevel analyses of individuals and cultures (pp. 249283). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R., Terracciano, A., & 79 Members of the Personality Profiles of Cultures Project. (2005). Personality profiles of cultures: Aggregate personality traits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 407425.Google Scholar
McWhorter, J. H. (2014). The language hoax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mesman, J., Minter, T., Angnged, A., Cissé, I. A. H., Salali, G. D., & Migliano, A. B. (2018). Universality without uniformity: A culturally inclusive approach to sensitive responsiveness in infant caregiving. Child Development, 89, 837850.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Laland, K. N. (2006). Towards a unified science of cultural evolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 329383.Google Scholar
Milanovic, B. (2009). Global inequality and the global inequality extraction ratio. New York: World Bank.Google Scholar
Milfont, T. L., & Klein, R. A. (2018). Replication and reproducibility in cross-cultural psychology. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 49, 735750.Google Scholar
Miyamoto, Y., & Kitayama, S. (2002). Cultural variation in correspondence bias: The critical role of attitude diagnosticity of socially constrained behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 12391248.Google Scholar
Mkhize, N. (2004). Psychology: An African perspective. In Ratele, K., Duncan, N., Hook, D., Mkhize, N., Kiguwa, P., & Collins, A. (Eds.), Self, community and psychology (pp. 129). Cape Town: UCT Press.Google Scholar
Morris, M. W., Chiu, C-Y. , & Liu, Z. (2015). Polycultural psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 631659.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moscovici, S. (1984). The phenomenon of social representations. In Farr, R. M. & Moscovici, S. (Eds.), Social representations (pp. 370). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Murata, A., Moser, J., & Kitayama, S. (2013). Culture shapes electrocortical responses during emotion suppression. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 595601.Google Scholar
Na, J., Grossmann, I., Varnum, M. E. W., Karasawa, M., Cho, Y., Kitayama, S., & Nisbett, R. E. (2020). Culture and personality revisited: Behavioral profiles and within‐person stability in interdependent (vs. independent) social orientation and holistic (vs. analytic) cognitive style. Journal of Personality, 88, 908924.Google Scholar
Na, J., Grossmann, I., Varnum, M. E. W., Kitayama, S., Gonzalez, R., & Nisbett, R. E. (2010). Cultural differences are not always reducible to individual differences. PNAS 107(14), 61926197.Google Scholar
Nel, J. A., Valchev, V. H., Rothmann, S., van de Vijver, F. J. R., Meiring, D., & de Bruin, G. P. (2012). Exploring the personality structure in the 11 languages of South Africa. Journal of Personality, 80(4), 915-948.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E. (2003). The geography of thought: How Asians and Westerners think differently … and why. New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K. P., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108, 291310.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A., Shariff, A. F., Gervais, W. M., Wilard, A. K., McNamara, R. A., Slingerland, E., & Henrich, J. (2014). The cultural evolution of prosocial religions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, ArtID: e1.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A., Smith, E. E., Kim, B. J., & Nisbett, R. E. (2002). Cultural preferences for formal versus intuitive reasoning. Cognitive Science, 26, 653684.Google Scholar
Nosek, B. A., Ebersole, C. R., DeHaven, A. C., & Mellor, D. T. (2018). The preregistration revolution. PNAS, 115, 26002606.Google Scholar
Nowak, M. A. (2006). Five rules for the evolution of cooperation. Science, 314(5805), 15601563.Google Scholar
Collaboration., Open Science (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science 349, aac4716.Google Scholar
Oxfam. (2015). Wealth: Having it all and wanting more. Oxford: Oxfam.Google Scholar
Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 372.Google Scholar
Oyserman, D., & Lee, S. W. S. (2008). Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 311342.Google Scholar
Panizza, F. (Ed.). (2005). Populism and the mirror of democracy. London: Verso.Google Scholar
Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (1999). Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. American Psychologist, 54, 741754.Google Scholar
Penn, D. C., Holyoak, K. J., & Povinelli, D. J. (2008). Darwin’s mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 109178.Google Scholar
Pettigrew, T. F. (2015). In pursuit of three theories: Authoritarianism, relative deprivation, and intergroup contact. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 121.Google Scholar
Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2006). A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 133.Google Scholar
Center, Pew Research (2017). What It Takes to Truly Be “One of Us.” Available from http://assets.pewresearch.org/wp-content/uploads/sites/2/2017/04/14094140/Pew-Research-Center-National-Identity-Report-FINAL-February-1-2017.pdf. Accessed April 14, 2018.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1970). Piaget’s theory. In Mussen, P. H. (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (3rd ed., pp. 703732). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Pick, S., & Sirkin, J. (2010). Breaking the cycle of poverty: The human basis for sustainable development. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Piketty, T. (2014). Capital in the twenty-first century. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.Google Scholar
Poortinga, Y. H. (1999). Do differences in behavior imply a need for different psychologies? Applied Psychology, An International Review, 48, 419432.Google Scholar
Poortinga, Y. H. (2003). Coherence of culture and generalizability of data: Two questionable assumptions in cross-cultural psychology. In Berman, J. & Berman, J. (Eds.), Cross-cultural differences in perspectives on the self. Vol. 49 of the Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 257–305). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Poortinga, Y. H. (2015). Is “culture” a workable concept for (cross-)cultural psychology? Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, 2(1).Google Scholar
Poortinga, Y. H., & Soudijn, K. (2002). Behavior-culture relationships and ontogenetic development. In Keller, H., Poortinga, Y. H., & Schölmerich, A. (Eds.), Biology, culture and development: Integrating diverse perspectives (pp. 320340). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Poortinga, Y. H., & Van Hemert, D. A. (2001). Personality and culture: Demarcating between the common and the unique. Journal of Personality, 69, 10331060.Google Scholar
Popper, K. R. (1959). The logic of scientific discovery. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Ray, V. F. (1952). Techniques and problems in the study of human color perception. South Western Journal of Anthropology, 8, 251259.Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Li, X. S., Williams, C. C., Cave, K. R., & Well, A. D. (2007). Eye movements during information processing tasks: Individual differences and cultural effects. Vision Research, 47, 27142726.Google Scholar
Reichenbach, H. (1938). Experience and prediction. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Rentfrow, P. J., & Jokela, M. (2016). Geographical psychology: The spatial organization of psychological phenomena. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25, 393398.Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Rivers, W. H. R. (1901). Vision. In Physiology and psychology, Part I. Reports of the Cambridge Anthropological Expedition to Torres Straits (Vol. II). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Roberson, D., Davidoff, J., Davies, I. R. L., & Shapiro, L. R. (2004). The development of color categories in two languages: A longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 554571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberson, D., Davies, I., & Davidoff, J. (2000). Color categories are not universal: Replications and new evidence from a stone-age culture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 369398.Google Scholar
Rogoff, D. (2014). Learning by observing and pitching in to family and community endeavors: An orientation. Human Development, 57, 150161.Google Scholar
Rosa, A., & Valsiner, J. (Eds). (2018). The Cambridge handbook of sociocultural psychology (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1991). Culture and the categorization of emotions. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 426450.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1994). Is there universal recognition of emotion from facial expression? A review of cross-cultural studies. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 102141.Google Scholar
Sahlins, M. (1977). The use and abuse of biology. London: Tavistock Publications.Google Scholar
Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., & Diener, E. (2005). Individualism: A valid and important dimension of cultural differences between nations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 9, 1731.Google Scholar
Schönpflug, U. (Ed.). (2009). Cultural transmission: Developmental, psychological, social and methodological perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schwandt, T. A., Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (2007). Judging interpretations: But is it rigorous? Trustworthiness and authenticity in naturalistic evaluation. New Directions for Evaluation, 114, 1125.Google Scholar
Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In Zanna, M. P. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 165). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Schwartz, S. H. (2012, Dec.). An overview of the Schwartz theory of basic values. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture. https://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/orpc/Google Scholar
Scribner, S. (1979). Modes of thinking and ways of speaking: Culture and logic reconsidered. In Freedle, R. O. (Ed.), New directions in discourse processing (pp. 223−243). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Scribner, S., & Cole, M. (1981). The psychology of literacy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Segall, M. H. (1996). Individualism and collectivism: Descriptions or explanations? [Review of the book Individualism and collectivism]. Contemporary Psychology, 41, 540542.Google Scholar
Segall, M. H., Campbell, D. T., & Herskovits, K. J. (1966). The influence of culture on visual perception. Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs-Merrill.Google Scholar
Segall, M. H., Dasen, P. R., Berry, J. W., & Poortinga, Y. H. (1990). Human behavior in global perspective: An introduction to cross-cultural psychology. New York: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Selig, J. P., Card, N. A., & Little, T. D. (2008). Latent structural equation modeling in cross-cultural research: Multigroup and multilevel approaches. In van de Vijver, F. J. R., van Hemert, D. A., & Poortinga, Y. H. (Eds.), Individuals and cultures in multilevel analysis (pp. 93119). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sen, A. (1999). Development as freedom. New York: Anchor Books.Google Scholar
Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A. (1984). Preview. In Shweder, R. A. & LeVine, R. A. (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion (pp. 124). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A. (1990). Cultural psychology – What is it? In J. Stigler, W., Shweder, R. A., & Herdt, G. (Eds.), Cultural psychology: Essays on comparative human development (pp. 143). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology: Undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychological Science, 22, 13591366.Google Scholar
Simons, R. C., & Hughes, C. C. (1985). Culture bound syndromes. In Gaw, A. C. (Ed.), Culture, ethnicity and mental illness (pp. 7593). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Press.Google Scholar
Singh, S., & Tripathi, R. C. (2010). Why do the bonded fear freedom? Some lessons from the field. Psychology and Developing Societies, 22, 249297.Google Scholar
Sinha, D. (1997). Indigenizing psychology. In Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., & Pandey, J. (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 129169). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Small, M. L., Harding, D. J., & Lamont, M. (2010). Reconsidering culture and poverty. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 629, 627.Google Scholar
Song, R., Moon, S., Chen, H., & Houston, M. B. (2018). When marketing strategy meets culture: The role of culture in product evaluations. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 46, 384402.Google Scholar
Sosis, R., & Bressler, E. (2003). Cooperation and commune longevity: A test of the costly signaling theory of religion. Cross-Cultural Research, 37, 211239.Google Scholar
Soudijn, K. A., Hutschemaekers, G. J. M., & Van de Vijver, F. J. R. (1990). Culture conceptualisations. In Van de Vijver, F. J. R. & Hutschemaekers, G. J. M. (Eds.), The investigation of culture: Current issues in cultural psychology (pp. 1939). Tilburg, the Netherlands: Tilburg University Press.Google Scholar
Spector, P. E., Cooper, C. L., & Sparks, K. (2001). An international study of the psychometric properties of the Hofstede values survey module 1994. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 50, 269281.Google Scholar
Steenkamp, J.-B. E. M., & Ter Hofstede, F. (2002). International market segmentation: Issues and perspectives. International Journal of Research in Marketing 19, 185213.Google Scholar
Super, C. M. (1976). Environmental effects on motor development: The case of “African infant precocity.Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 18, 561567.Google Scholar
Super, C. M., & Harkness, S. (1986). The developmental niche: A conceptualization at the interface of child and culture. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 9, 545569.Google Scholar
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In Austin, W. G. & Worchel, S. (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 3347). Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
Takano, Y., & Sogon, S. (2008). Are Japanese more collectivistic than Americans? Examining conformity in in-groups and the reference-group effect. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39, 237250.Google Scholar
The Illusions Index. See www.illusionsindex.org/ir/vertical-horizontal-illusion (consulted August 2020).Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift fuer Tierpsychologie, 20, 410433.Google Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990). The past explains the present: Emotional adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments. Ethology and Sociobiology, 11, 375424.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cultural contexts. Psychological Review, 96, 506520.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1996). The psychological measurement of cultural syndromes. American Psychologist, 51, 407415.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 3557.Google Scholar
Tsai, J. L., & Clobert, M. (2019). Cultural influences on emotion: Established patterns and emerging trends. In Cohen, D. & Kitayama, S. (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (2nd ed., pp. 292318). New York: Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Tylor, E. B. (1958). Primitive culture. New York: Harper (first published 1871).Google Scholar
United Nations. (2017). Resolution adopted by the General Assembly on 6 July 2017.Google Scholar
Valchev, V. H., Van de Vijver, F. J. R., Meiring, D., Nel, J. A., Hill, C., Laher, S. & Adams, B. G. (2014). Beyond Agreeableness: Social–relational personality concepts from an indigenous and cross-cultural perspective. Journal of Research in Personality 48, 1732.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (Ed.). (2012). The Oxford handbook of culture and psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Van de Schoot, R., Kaplan, D., Denissen, J., Asendorpf, J. B. Neyer, F. J., & Van Aken, M. A. G. (2014). A gentle introduction to Bayesian analysis: Applications to developmental research. Child Development, 85, 842860.Google Scholar
Van de Vijver, F. J. R., & Leung, K. (1997). Methods and data analysis for cross-cultural research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Van de Vijver, F. J. R., & Leung, K. (2021). Methods and data analysis for cross-cultural research (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Van de Vijver, F. J. R., Chasiotis, A., & Breugelmans, S. M. (Eds.) (2011). Fundamental questions in cross-cultural psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Van de Vijver, F. J. R., & Poortinga, Y. H. (1997). Towards an integrated analysis of bias in cross-cultural assessment. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 13, 2129.Google Scholar
Van de Vijver, F. J. R., & Poortinga, Y. H. (2020). Dealing with methodological pitfalls in cross-cultural studies of stress. In Ringeisen, T., Genkova, P., & Leong, F. (Eds.), Handbuch Stress und Kultur. [Handbook stress and culture]. Wiesbaden: Springer.Google Scholar
Van de Vijver, F. J. R., Avvisati, F., Davidov, E., Eid, M., Fox, J.-P., Le Donné, N. … Van de Schoot, R. (2019). Invariance analyses in large-scale studies. OECD Education Working Paper No. 201. Paris: OECD.Google Scholar
Van de Vijver, F. J. R., Blommaert, J. M. E., Gkoumasi, G., & Stogianni, M. (2015). On the need to broaden the concept of ethnic identity. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 46, 3646.Google Scholar
Van de Vijver, F. J. R., Van Hemert, D. A., & Poortinga, Y. H. (Eds.). (2008). Individuals and cultures in multi-level analysis (pp. 326). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Van Hemert, D. A., Van de Vijver, F. J. R., & Poortinga, Y. H. (2002). The Beck Depression Inventory as a measure of subjective well-being: A cross-national study. Journal of Happiness Studies, 3, 257286.Google Scholar
Verkuyten, M. (2014). Identity and cultural diversity: What social psychology can teach us. London: Routledge/Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Vertovec, S. (2007). Super-diversity and its implications. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 30, 10241054.Google Scholar
Vignoles, V. L. Owe, E., Becker, M., Smith, P. B., Easterbrook, M. J., Brown, R. … Bond, M. H. (2016). Beyond the “East–West” dichotomy: Global variation in cultural models of selfhood. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 9661000.Google Scholar
Voland, E., Chasiotis, A., & Schievenhövel, W. (Eds.). (2005). Grandmotherhood. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.Google Scholar
Vul, E., Harris, C., Winkielman, P., & Pashler, H. (2009). Puzzlingly high correlations in fMRI studies of emotion, personality, and social cognition. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 274290.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Walter, K. V., Conroy-Beam, D., Buss, D. M., Asao, K., Sorokowska, A., Sorokowski, P. … Zupančič, M. (2020). Sex differences in mate preferences across 45 countries: A large-scale replication. Psychological Science, 31, 408423.Google Scholar
Ward, C. (2008). Thinking outside the Berry boxes: New perspectives on identity, acculturation and intercultural relations. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 32, 105114.Google Scholar
Wen, H., & Wang, H. (2013). Confucian cultural psychology and its contextually creative intentionality. Culture & Psychology, 19, 184202.Google Scholar
Whiten, A. (2021). The psychological reach of culture in animals’ lives. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 30, 211217.Google Scholar
Whiting, J. W. M. (1994). Environmental constraints on infant care practices. In Chasdi, E. H. (Ed.), Culture and human development (pp. 107134). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought and reality. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wicherts, J. M., Veldkamp, C. L. S., Augusteijn, H. E. M., Bakker, M., van Aert, R. C. M., & van Assen, M. A. L. M. (2016). Degrees of freedom in planning, running, analyzing, and reporting psychological studies: A checklist to avoid p-hacking. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, ArtID: 1832.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, R. G., & Pickett, K. E. (2017). The enemy between us: The psychological and social costs of inequality. European Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 1124.Google Scholar
Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology: The new synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, H. F. (2017). On geography and encounter: Bodies, borders, and difference. Progress in Human Geography, 41, 451471.Google Scholar
Wong, B. I., Yin, S., Yang, L., Li, J. & Spaniol, J. (2018). Cultural differences in memory for objects and backgrounds in pictures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 49, 404417.Google Scholar
Woodworth, R. S., & Schlosberg, H. (1954). Experimental psychology (rev. ed.). New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Worthman, C. M. (2010). The ecology of human development: Evolving models for cultural psychology. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41, 546562.Google Scholar
Wundt, W. (1913). Elemente der Völkerpsychologie (2nd edn.) [Elements of Völkerpsychologie]. Leipzig: Alfred Kroner Verlag.Google Scholar
Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (1997). The handicap principle: A missing piece of Darwin’s puzzle. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Save element to Kindle

To save this element to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Concept and Method in Cross-Cultural and Cultural Psychology
  • Ype H. Poortinga, Tilburg University, The Netherlands
  • Online ISBN: 9781108908320
Available formats
×

Save element to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Concept and Method in Cross-Cultural and Cultural Psychology
  • Ype H. Poortinga, Tilburg University, The Netherlands
  • Online ISBN: 9781108908320
Available formats
×

Save element to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Concept and Method in Cross-Cultural and Cultural Psychology
  • Ype H. Poortinga, Tilburg University, The Netherlands
  • Online ISBN: 9781108908320
Available formats
×