Elsevier

Psychiatry Research

Volume 230, Issue 2, 15 December 2015, Pages 172-180
Psychiatry Research

Correlation between neuropsychological and social cognition measures and symptom dimensions in schizophrenic patients

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.08.034Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Disorganized dimension has the most significant impact on cognition.

  • Disorganized dimension is separated from cognitive dimension clinically evaluated.

  • Ecological tasks seem a valid tool in evaluating cognition in SKZ.

Abstract

Neurocognitive and social cognition deficits have been largely reported in Schizophrenia (SKZ) but their association with psychopathology remains uncertain. Our purpose was to explore the relationship between symptom dimensions and neuropsychological performances.

We enrolled 35 stabilized schizophrenic outpatients of the Department of Psychiatry of Policlinico Hospital, University of Milan, who completed psychiatric Rating Scales, the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS) and the Executive and Social Cognition Battery (ESCB).

Disorganized dimension seems to have the most significant impact on cognition, being associated with performance in several BACS subtests (verbal memory, working memory, motor speed, symbol coding, Tower of London) and ESCB tasks (MET and Hotel task number of tasks attempted, number of broken MET rules, sum of deviations in Hotel Task). Positive dimension correlated with performance in verbal fluency, negative dimension with IOWA Test results, cognitive dimension with MET number of inefficiencies and Eyes test score. Impulsive-aggressive and depressive dimensions weakly correlated only with Faux Pas test. Our study supports the existence of a specific disorganized dimension in SKZ, separated from cognitive dimension evaluated through clinical instruments (e.g. PANSS), but capable of influencing cognitive abilities. Furthermore, it strengthens the validity of ecological tasks in evaluating cognition in SKZ.

Introduction

Schizophrenia (SKZ) is no longer considered a single disease entity, but a heterogeneous clinical syndrome, with multiple and different clinical manifestations (Tandon, 2012, Tandon et al., 2013). The SKZ definition substantially changed with the DSM-5 (APA, 2013) giving space to a dimensional approach that should overcome poor explanation of high heterogeneity provided by DSM-IV (APA, 2000) subtypes (Barch et al., 2013, Heckers et al., 2013).

Neurocognitive deficits and negative symptoms received the strongest support as related to major processes (Green et al., 2000, Kirkpatrick et al., 2006). In particular the degree of cognitive impairment is predictive of social skills (Barch and Keefe, 2010) and seems to remain unchanged despite the remission of acute symptoms (San et al., 2007).

Social Cognition (SC), which involves a complex set of processes allowing adaptive social interaction (Green et al., 2005) and in particular one of its aspects, the Theory of Mind (ToM), has repeatedly shown to be compromised in most SKZ patients (Lee et al., 2004). ‘’Mentalizing” is conceptualized as the ability to reflect upon one's own and other people's mental states including desires, beliefs, knowledge, intentions and feelings (Frith and Frith, 2003). Brune (2005) demonstrated that ToM is crucially linked to social behavioral competence and is the most significant predictor of severe social behavioral abnormalities regardless of the duration of illness.

However, explicit knowledge could not be enough for a good performance in real life, because an individual may demonstrate a good functional capacity but may not be able to use it in his own social context (Bromley and Brekke, 2010). For this reason to assess neurocognitive performance several studies used some “ecological” tasks, in order to obtain more context-sensitive measures similar to real-life situations (Baez et al., 2013, Caletti et al., 2013).

One of the most commonly used rating scales in assessing psychopathological symptoms in SKZ is the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale for Schizophrenia (PANSS) (Kay et al., 1987). Several studies attempted to rearrange PANSS items through factor analysis, trying to obtain new models capable of measuring illness dimensions more accurately (Peralta and Cuesta, 1994, Emsley et al., 2003). Most of them obtained five factor models with a high similarity in items distribution (Lykouras et al., 2000). In particular, Wallwork et al. (2012) attempted to build a consensus model by assigning only the consistent PANSS items to each of the five factors based on previously published models. The relationship between these five components and the socio-demographic and clinical characteristics has allowed to identify two variants of SKZ: one characterized by a prevalent florid symptomatology, with fewer negative symptoms and cognitive disturbances, the other with a predominant cognitive impairment and small number of positive and negative symptoms. The former variant seemed to be associated with reduced hospitalization (Lindenmayer et al., 1995, Lykouras et al., 2000).

Other studies tried to investigate the correlation between illness dimensions and cognition and demonstrated that an alteration of processing speed is seen to indirectly affect negative symptoms (Lipkovich et al., 2009). In general, an association between cognition and negative symptoms have been found, but in different degrees (Green and Nuechterlein, 1999, Lewandowski et al., 2011, Bagney et al., 2013) rendering the correlation uncertain. The high presence of disorganized thinking and/or formal alterations of thought should be intuitively linked to greater cognitive impairment, but also in this case the results are uncertain (Pandina et al., 2013, Rodríguez-Jiménez et al., 2013) and in addition, the same symptoms seem to moderate the pathway between cognition and both social cognition and metacognition (Minor and Lysaker, 2014). Moreover, a correlation between positive and depressive dimensions and cognitive deficits seems weak (Dominguez Mde et al., 2009, Yilmaz et al., 2012).

Psychopathological dimensions and SC (e.g. emotional processing, ToM, perception of social relationship) have been investigated but still require a deeper understanding (Ventura et al., 2011, Lam et al., 2014). Couture et al. (2006) showed that SC deficits are an important factor in the progress of various types of symptoms and might be a mediator of the association between neurocognition and outcome. ToM outcome, which seems to be an independent factor from executive functions tout court (Pickup, 2008), was found to be related to disorganized symptoms and disturbances of thinking, paranoid delusions (Harrington et al., 2005) and increased severity of positive and negative symptoms (Harrington et al., 2005, Brüne et al., 2007, Green et al., 2008, Green and Horan, 2010, Abdel-Hamid et al., 2009, Ventura et al., 2010, Urbach et al., 2013).

Since a degree of neurocognitive impairment has been shown to be predictive of social skills such as management of daily life in SKZ patients, our study tried to understand the relationship between SKZ symptom dimensions and neuropsychological and SC processes. We hypothesized a correlation between principal psychopathological dimensions and neurocognitive performance, stronger for disorganized dimension and persistent even when neuropsychological and SC performances are evaluated with ecological tasks.

Section snippets

Subjects

Thirty-five SKZ outpatients (aged 19–63) enrolled at the Department of Psychiatry of the University of Milan. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Fondazione IRCCS Ca’ Granda Maggiore Policlinico Hospital and informed consent was obtained from all subjects.

Inclusion criteria

  • A diagnosis of SKZ according to DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000).

  • No substance abuse for at least two weeks.

  • Clinical stability during last 6 weeks and treatment with stabilizing doses of antipsychotics.

Exclusion criteria

Results

30 SKZ patients (6 males, 24 females) according to DSM-IV criteria (10 Paranoid, 6 Disorganized and 14 Indifferentiated types) were included in the sample.

Age was between 19 and 63 years (mean value 42.47±10.4 years). Mean age at onset was 21.2±3.99 years (earliest onset 13 years old, latest 30). Mean duration of illness was 21.27±11.59 years and mean duration of untreated illness (DUI) was 3.23±5.04 years. Only six patients had a duration of untreated illness of 10 years or more. 19 in 30

Discussion

Disorganized dimension seems to have the most significant impact on neurocognitive functions, even when evaluated with ESCB tasks. This is in agreement with most of the studies which have considered the correlation between neurocognitive functioning and symptom dimensions. Overall, these studies founding a link between the dimension “disorganization” and the failure in various neuropsychological tests (Daban et al., 2002, Klingberg et al., 2006, Johnson et al., 2009). In particular, when

Conflict of interest

A.C. Altamura has served as a consultant on Advisory Boards for Roche, Lundbeck, Merck, Astra Zeneca, Bristol Myers Squibb, Janssen-Cilag, Sanofi, Eli Lilly, Pfizer and Otsuka.

Other authors declare no conflict of interest.

Contributors

E. Caletti and S. Zago designed the study. E. Caletti, E. Zugno, M. Cigliobianco, A. Caldiroli and C. Prunas collected the data and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. P. Grillo analyzed the data. A.C. Altamura and R.A. Paoli supervised collection of data and assisted in manuscript composition.

All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the participants for their involvement and dedication to this research.

This work was not supported by any grant awards.

References (109)

  • M. Brüne et al.

    Mental state attribution, neurocognitive functioning, and psychopathology: what predicts poor social competence in schizophrenia best?

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2007)
  • E. Brunet-Gouet et al.

    The study of social cognition with neuroimaging methods as a means to explore future directions of deficit evaluation in schizophrenia?

    Psychiatry Res.

    (2011)
  • G. Collin et al.

    Symptom dimensions are associated with progressive brain volume changes in schizophrenia

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2012)
  • A.M. Cusi et al.

    Theory of mind deficits in patients with mild symptoms of major depressive disorder

    Psychiatry Res.

    (2013)
  • C. Daban et al.

    Correlation between clinical syndromes and neuropsychological tasks in unmedicated patients with recent onset schizophrenia

    Psychiatry Res.

    (2002)
  • R. Emsley et al.

    The factor structure for the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) in recent-onset psychosis

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2003)
  • V.M. Goghari et al.

    The functional neuroanatomy of symptom dimensions in schizophrenia: a qualitative and quantitative review of a persistent question

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2010)
  • K.P. Good et al.

    The relationship of neuropsychological test performance with the PANSS in antipsychotic naive, first-episode psychosis patients

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2004)
  • S. Heckers et al.

    Structure of the psychotic disorders classification in DSM-5

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2013)
  • F. Irani et al.

    Self-face recognition and theory of mind in patients with schizophrenia and first-degree relatives

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2006)
  • R.S.E. Keefe et al.

    The Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive battery

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2004)
  • L. Lee et al.

    Mental state decoding abilities in clinical depression

    J. Affect. Disord.

    (2005)
  • K.E. Lewandowski et al.

    Relationship of neurocognitive deficits to diagnosis and symptoms across affective and non-affective psychoses

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2011)
  • J.P. Lindenmayer et al.

    Five factor model of schizophrenia: replication across samples

    Schizophr. Res.

    (1995)
  • F. Manes et al.

    “Real life” executive deficits in patients with focal vascular lesions affecting the cerebellum

    J. Neurol. Sci.

    (2009)
  • T. Manly et al.

    Rehabilitation of executive function: facilitation of effective goal management on complex tasks using periodic auditory alerts

    Neuropsychologia

    (2002)
  • M.B. Meyer et al.

    Elementary neurocognitive function, facial affect recognition and social-skills in schizophrenia

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2009)
  • K.S. Minor et al.

    Necessary, but not sufficient: links between neurocognition, social cognition, and metacognition in schizophrenia are moderated by disorganized symptoms

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2014)
  • G. Pandina et al.

    Identification of clinically meaningful relationships among cognition, functionality, and symptoms in subjects with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2013)
  • V. Peralta et al.

    Psychometric properties of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) in schizophrenia

    Psychiatry Res.

    (1994)
  • M. Poletti et al.

    Cognitive and affective Theory of Mind in neurodegenerative diseases: Neuropsychological, neuroanatomical and neurochemical levels

    Neurosci. Behav. Rev.

    (2012)
  • R.A. Richell et al.

    Theory of mind and psychopathy: can psychopathic individuals read the “language of the eyes”?

    Neuropsychologia

    (2003)
  • T. Rietkerk et al.

    The genetic of symptom dimensions of schizophrenia: review and meta-analysis

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2008)
  • V.M. Rodriguez et al.

    Schizophrenic syndromes and clozapine response in treatment-resistant schizophrenia

    Psychiatry Res.

    (1998)
  • R. Rodríguez-Jiménez et al.

    Cognition and the five-factor model of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale in schizophrenia

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2013)
  • L. San et al.

    Symptomatic remission and social/vocational functioning in outpatients with schizophrenia: prevalence and associations in a cross-sectional study

    Eur. Psychiatry

    (2007)
  • B. Sans-Sansa et al.

    Association of formal thought disorder in schizophrenia with structural brain abnormalities in language-related cortical regions

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2013)
  • M.J. Sergi et al.

    Social cognition in schizophrenia: Relationships with neurocognition and negative symptoms

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2007)
  • R. Tandon

    The nosology of Schizophrenia. Toward DSM – 5 and ICD – 11

    Psychiatry Clin. N. Am.

    (2012)
  • R. Tandon et al.

    Definition and description of schizophrenia in the DSM-5

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2013)
  • D. Addington et al.

    Assessing depression in schizophrenia: the Calgary Depression Scale

    Br. J. Psychiatry

    (1993)
  • R. Adolphs et al.

    Impaired recognition of social emotions following amygdale damage

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (2002)
  • American Psychiatric Association

    Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition

    (2000)
  • American Psychiatric Association

    Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition

    (2013)
  • S. Anselmetti et al.

    The Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia. Normative data for the Italian population

    Neurol. Sci.

    (2008)
  • S. Baez et al.

    Contextual social cognition impairments in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder

    PLoS One

    (2013)
  • A. Bagney et al.

    Negative symptoms and executive function in schizophrenia: does their relationship change with illness duration?

    Psychopathology

    (2013)
  • D.M. Barch et al.

    Anticipating DSM-V: opportunities and challenges for cognition and psychosis

    Schizophr. Bull.

    (2010)
  • S. Baron-Cohen et al.

    Another advanced test of Theory of Mind: Evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome

    J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry

    (1997)
  • S. Baron-Cohen et al.

    A new test of social sensitivity: detection of faux pas in normal children and children with Asperger syndrome

    J. Autism Dev. Disord.

    (1999)
  • Cited by (13)

    • Stability of Verbal Fluency in Outpatients with Schizophrenia

      2021, Psychiatry Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      As previously indicated, significant heterogeneity exists in schizophrenia with respect to neurocognitive impairment. There are robust findings, however, that indicate impairments in verbal fluency (i.e., spontaneous, verbal word production) are reliably demonstrated by most patients with the illness (Altamura, et al., 2015; Chang, et al., 2013; Erol et al., 2012; Heinrichs and Zakzanis, 1998; Helmes and Hall, 2015; Kim, et al., 2015; Kremen et al., 2003; Matsuda, et al., 2013). There is further evidence to suggest that deficits in verbal fluency may have a genetic underpinning, as this has been identified in first-degree relatives (Erol et al., 2012; Kim et al., 2015; Ozan et al., 2010) and individuals at risk of experiencing a first episode psychosis (Cochrane et al., 2012).

    • Theory of mind and schizophrenia in young and middle-aged patients: Influence of executive functions

      2018, Psychiatry Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      The latter is a multidimensional construct comprising emotional processing, social perception and knowledge, attribution bias and theory of mind (ToM), defined as the ability to understand that others also have minds, with different and separate mental states from our own (Green et al., 2005). ToM has been shown to be compromised in schizophrenia (Altamura et al., 2015), relating to disorganized symptoms, paranoid delusions and severe positive and negative symptoms (Harrington et al., 2005; Mancuso et al., 2011; Ventura et al., 2010; Martinez et al., 2017). It has been viewed as an important factor in predicting social disability (Brüne et al., 2007).

    • Cognitive remediation and social cognitive training for violence in schizophrenia: a systematic review

      2017, Psychiatry Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      The disabled "emotional vigil" system and violent inhibition, as well as the failure to identify the salient and informative features in other people's faces, are the main elements leading to violent acts. The theory of mind, which includes cognitive empathy, is statistically and closely connected with aggression and severe disruptive behaviors in SCZ (Altamura et al., 2015) and with physical aggression in paranoid SCZ (Chan, 2012). This could involve misperceptions or even a total lack of perception of other people's intentions, thus producing cognitive biases that prevent the resolution of interpersonal conflicts.

    • Determinants of cerebral hemodynamics during the Trail Making Test in schizophrenia

      2016, Brain and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      In accordance with the lack of correlation in performance between the TMT-A and TMT-B in schizophrenia, we suggest that cognitive performance does not reflect the same underlying mechanism as in healthy subjects (Ojeda et al., 2010). Higher BPRS and CGI scores were associated with decreased performance during the Trail Making Test, Part B, a rather intuitive result that fits well with the available literature (Altamura et al., 2015). Some limitations of our study are inherent to the Doppler method.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text