Elsevier

Neuropsychologia

Volume 51, Issue 13, November 2013, Pages 2930-2938
Neuropsychologia

Learning to detect but not to grasp suppressed visual stimuli

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.035Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Implication of the two-visual-systems hypothesis was tested in healthy participants.

  • Can the dorsal system use invisible information, whereas the ventral system cannot?

  • Participants grasped for invisible objects and reported their properties verbally.

  • Chance performance but decreasing perceptual thresholds in grasping task.

  • Perceptual performance possibly enhanced by concurrent visuomotor behavior.

Abstract

A central implication of the two-visual-systems hypothesis (TVSH) is that the dorsal visuomotor system (vision-for-action) can make use of invisible information, whereas the ventral system (vision-for-perception) cannot (Milner & Goodale, 1995). Therefore, actions such as grasping movements should be influenced by invisible information while conscious reports remain unaffected. To test this assumption, we used a dichoptic stimulation technique – continuous flash suppression (CFS) – which has the potency to render stimuli invisible for up to seconds (Tsuchiya & Koch, 2005). In two experiments using CFS, participants were asked to grasp for invisible bars of different sizes (Experiment 1) or orientations (Experiment 2), or to report both measures verbally. Target visibility was measured trial-by-trial using the perceptual awareness scale (PAS). We found no evidence for the use of invisible information by the visuomotor system despite extensive training (600 trials) and the availability of haptic feedback. Participants neither learned to scale their maximum grip aperture to the size of the invisible stimulus, nor to align their hand to its orientation. Careful control of stimulus visibility across training sessions, however, revealed a robust tendency towards decreasing perceptual thresholds under CFS. We discuss our results within the framework of the TVSH and with respect to alternative models which emphasize the close functional interaction between the dorsal and ventral visual systems.

Introduction

The ‘two visual systems’ hypothesis (TVSH) proposed by Milner and Goodale (1995) remains one of the most influential models of visual processing in the primate brain. It assumes a dissociation between consciously accessible ‘vision-for-perception’, mediated by the ventral cortical pathway, and consciously inaccessible ‘vision-for-action’, mediated by the dorsal pathway (Milner, 2012). One of the primary pillars this model rests on are the studies performed on patient D.F., who suffers from visual form agnosia as a result of extensive bilateral ventral stream damage following carbon monoxide poisoning (James et al., 2003, Milner et al., 1991; also see: Karnath, Rueter, Mandler, & Himmelbach, 2009). D.F. can successfully grasp for objects while she is unable to perform perceptual judgements such as size or shape estimation on them and her remaining visual capacities have been attributed to dorsal stream processes (Culham et al., 2003, Milner and Goodale, 2008).

In neurologically healthy participants, the demonstration of perception-action dissociations has proven to be difficult. A first study that showed that grasping (taken as a measure for dorsal processing) may be immune to the Ebbinghaus size illusion while perceptual measures were not (Aglioti, DeSouza, & Goodale, 1995) has prompted a multitude of studies, many of which do not support a dissociation (e.g., Franz, Gegenfurtner, Bülthoff, & Fahle, 2000; for reviews see Franz and Gegenfurtner, 2008, Westwood and Goodale, 2011). Another way to probe this dissociation in healthy participants has been paved by the introduction of a new technique called continuous flash suppression (CFS) which can render stimuli invisible for up to several seconds by flashing high-contrast images to one eye while showing the target stimulus to the other eye (Tsuchiya and Koch, 2005, Tsuchiya et al., 2006).

Evidence from behavioral priming experiments (Almeida et al., 2008, Sakuraba et al., 2012) and neuroimaging studies (Sterzer et al., 2008, Troiani et al., 2012) shows that stimulus information suppressed from awareness with CFS can reach higher-order visual areas and influence behavior.

It has been suggested that CFS could be used to “isolate” dorsal visual processing, i.e., to leave nonconscious visuomotor processes mediated by the dorsal stream intact while disrupting conscious perception mediated by the ventral stream (Fang and He, 2005, Lin and He, 2009; but see: Hesselmann and Malach, 2011, Hesselmann, 2013). If this was the case, one could expect participants to show above chance performance in grasping invisible stimuli, while not being able to report the stimuli's features verbally. Such behavior would classify as a form of “blindsight”, a rare dissociation in patients with lesions to primary visual cortex (V1), typically defined as residual forced-choice visual function in the absence of subjective awareness (Cowey, 2004, Stoerig and Cowey, 2007). According to the taxonomy for different blindsight subtypes proposed by Danckert & Rossetti, accurately acting upon blind field stimuli based on preserved activity in the dorsal stream classifies as “action-blindsight” (Danckert & Rossetti, 2005). We reasoned that testing for a similar perception-action dissociation in healthy observers using CFS would constitute a critical test for the TVSH, in particular its claim that ventral stream processes are associated with visual awareness, while dorsal processes are not. In this study, we aimed to generate a blindsight-like situation in normal observers to test whether information unavailable to conscious report would still be processed in a way to enable grasping for objects of different sizes (Experiment 1) or orientations (Experiment 2).

Section snippets

Participants

All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, were naïve to the purpose of the study, and were either paid for participation or received course credit. Procedures conformed to local ethics guidelines and all observers gave informed written consent. Five participants (two female) took part in Experiment 1. Their mean age was 23.8 years (range 19–30). They scored on average 86.82 (range 66.67–100) on the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory and were thus all right-handed. Two further

Experiment 1

In Experiment 1 we first presented visible stimuli and had participants grasp (session 1) or verbally estimate (session 2) the size of the stimuli. Both measures clearly scaled with object size (t4=8.39, p=0.001, t4=9.25, p<0.001). Then we rendered the stimuli invisible by CFS. This suppressed visibility and the participants' ability to react to the stimuli in a similar way in the verbal estimates and in grasping; neither was there significant scaling with object size in the ‘invisible’

Discussion

Our results show that rendering stimuli invisible with CFS caused participants' performance to fall to chance level in both grasping and verbal report tasks. Thus, our data are in disagreement with the hypothesis that the dorsal vision-for-action system can make use of invisible information, whereas the ventral vision-for-perception system cannot (Milner & Goodale, 1995).

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Isabel Roll, Christian Floß, and Timo Richter for their support and assistance with this project. G.H. and V.H.F. were supported by the German Research Foundation Grant (HE 6244/1-1). K.L. was supported by the Studienstiftung des deutschen Volkes (German National Academic Foundation). G.H. and V.H.F. contributed equally to this study.

References (63)

  • Z. Lin et al.

    Seeing the invisible: The scope and limits of unconscious processing in binocular rivalry

    Progress in Neurobiology

    (2009)
  • A.D. Milner et al.

    Does grasping in patient D.F. depend on vision?

    Trends in Cognitive Sciences

    (2012)
  • A.D. Milner et al.

    Two visual systems re-viewed

    Neuropsychologia

    (2008)
  • W. Roseboom et al.

    Learning to reach for ‘invisible’ visual input

    Current Biology

    (2011)
  • W. Roseboom et al.

    Erratum: Learning to reach for “invisible” visual input

    Current Biology

    (2013)
  • M. Rothkirch et al.

    A direct oculomotor correlate of unconscious visual processing

    Current Biology

    (2012)
  • K. Sandberg et al.

    Measuring consciousness: Is one measure better than the other?

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2010)
  • A. Schurger

    A very inexpensive MRI-compatible method for dichoptic visual stimulation

    Journal of Neuroscience Methods

    (2009)
  • P. Stoerig et al.

    Blindsight

    Current Biology

    (2007)
  • D.A. Westwood et al.

    Converging evidence for diverging pathways: neuropsychology and psychophysics tell the same story

    Vision Research

    (2011)
  • J. Almeida et al.

    Unconscious processing dissociates along categorical lines

    PNAS

    (2008)
  • D.H. Brainard

    The psychophysics toolbox

    Spatial Vision

    (1997)
  • C. Cavina-Pratesi et al.

    The magic grasp: Motor expertise in deception

    PLOS One

    (2011)
  • A. Cowey

    The 30th Sir Frederick Bartlett lecture: Fact, artefact, and myth about blindsight

    The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (2004)
  • J.C. Culham et al.

    Visually guided grasping produces fMRI activation in dorsal but not ventral stream brain areas

    Experimental Brain Research

    (2003)
  • F. Fang et al.

    Cortical responses to invisible objects in the human dorsal and ventral pathways

    Nature Neuroscience

    (2005)
  • V.H. Franz et al.

    Grasping visual illusions: Consistent data and no dissociation

    Cognitive Neuropsychology

    (2008)
  • V.H. Franz et al.

    Grasping visual illusions: No evidence for a dissociation between perception and action

    Psychological Science

    (2000)
  • M.A. Garcia-Perez

    A cautionary note on the use of the adaptive up-down method

    Journal of the Acoustical Society of America

    (2011)
  • C.L. Gonzalez et al.

    Hemispheric specialization for the visual control of action is independent of handedness

    Journal of Neurophysiology

    (2006)
  • M.A. Goodale

    Action without perception in human vision

    Cognitive Neuropsychology

    (2008)
  • Cited by (36)

    • Seeing food fast and slow: Arousing pictures and words have reverse priorities in accessing awareness

      2022, Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      First, we recruited different groups of participants to verify the effect(s) of food pictures (Experiment 1), food words (Experiment 2), diffeomorphic pictures (Experiment 3), and binocular-viewing conditions (Experiment 4). An advantage of recruiting different participants across experiments is to decrease the practice effect of Mondrian masks, where the target detection threshold will decrease after hundreds of trials of training (Ludwig, Sterzer, Kathmann, Franz, & Hesselmann, 2013). A within-subject design might be able to decrease the group variance.

    • Conscious perception of flickering stimuli in binocular rivalry and continuous flash suppression is not affected by tACS-induced SSR modulation

      2020, Consciousness and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      Next, we asked if time was also a significant predictor or RT and found a negative relationship, such that training led to a lower RT (Formula: RT = 2.40–0.19 * block, F(1, 70) = 8.80, p = 0.004, R2 = 0.10; Fig. 5b). Both these results together point out that more time elapsing led to better performance (Ludwig, Sterzer, Kathmann, Franz, & Hesselmann, 2013). Type I and II AUC were computed for each participant individually.

    • Individual differences in continuous flash suppression: Potency and linkages to binocular rivalry dynamics

      2019, Vision Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Although not the main purpose of our study, our data provide an excellent opportunity to examine whether CFS potency as indexed by percentage of bulls-eye breakthroughs varied over the course of the four prolonged episodes of CFS deprivation. Results from an earlier study involving an adaptive training procedure (Ludwig, Sterzer, Kathmann, Franz, & Hesselmann, 2013) found that a target pitted against a fixed amplitude CFS mask had to be reduced in contrast to maintain “full invisibility” of that target over repeated trials, implying that the strength of CFS wanes over trials. We have looked for direct evidence of this trend in our large sample of participants tested on four successive occasions using the same procedures.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    Equal contribution.

    View full text