Elsevier

Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews

Volume 116, September 2020, Pages 221-238
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews

Neural correlates of manual action language: Comparative review, ALE meta-analysis and ROI meta-analysis

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.06.025Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We compared action language to motor observation, imagery and execution.

  • A systematic review, meta-analysis and ROI analysis were conducted.

  • Action language is more similar to observation than to imagery and execution.

  • The core network for motor execution is not involved in action language processing.

Abstract

Despite decades of research, the nature of the involvement of the motor system in action language processing is still controversial, and little is known about how processing action language relates to observing, imaging and executing motor actions. This study combines a systematic review of the literature, an ALE meta-analysis and a region-of-interest (ROI) meta-analysis to provide the first complete (qualitative and quantitative) account of the motor-related functional network involved in action language processing in comparison to activation reported during motor observation, motor imagery and motor execution. The review of the literature revealed that the methodology of action language studies differed considerably from the methodology of other motor-related processes which may have contributed to blurring the interpretations over the years. The ALE and ROI meta-analyses showed that the functional network of action language was more similar to observation than imagery and finally execution, following a motor gradation. Overall, our results point towards a more cognitive, as opposed to purely motoric, involvement of the motor system during action language processing.

Introduction

The cerebral network of action language has been the focus of extensive research in the last decades and a wide range of theories on language embodiment have been developed (e.g. Binder and Desai, 2011; Gallese and Lakoff, 2005; Glenberg, 2015; Mahon and Caramazza, 2008; Pulvermüller, 2013; Zwaan, 2014). However, the specific role of the motor system in action language processing and the extent to which action language is embodied remain controversial (Meteyard et al., 2012; Zwaan, 2014). Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have played an important part in demonstrating the involvement of the motor system during the processing of action language (e.g.Hauk et al., 2004; Kemmerer et al., 2008; Labruna et al., 2011; van Dam et al., 2012). Evidence has accumulated that suggests that the relationship between action language and the motor system varies as a function of several factors, including context, expertise, attentional focus and semantic control demand (Davey, 2015; Moody and Gennari, 2010; Yang, 2014; Zwaan, 2014). This suggests that the involvement of the motor system in action language processing is complex but important questions remain. What is the nature of this motor involvement? What are the motor-related neural mechanisms at play during action language? Of particular interest are the studies that directly compared action language with other motor-related processes. Such comparisons have revealed similarities between the action language network and the networks of motor observation (Aziz-Zadeh et al., 2006; Meister and Iacoboni, 2007), motor imagery (Yang and Shu, 2014) and motor execution (Moody-Triantis et al., 2014; Papeo et al., 2012; Peck et al., 2009). However, a lack of overlap between the neural networks of action language and these other motor-related processes has also been reported (Tomasino et al., 2007; Tremblay and Small, 2011; Willems et al., 2009; Zhang et al., 2018). Thus, agreement about the relationship between the neural networks supporting action language processing and those supporting other motor-related processes has not been reached yet.

Meta-analyses provide a quantitative methodology to summarize empirical data, which may help in reaching a consensus. Meta-analyses have been conducted to characterize the cerebral network of action language (Jirak et al., 2010), motor observation (Caspers et al., 2010) and motor imagery (Hétu et al., 2013), providing a comprehensive overview of the neural networks involved in each of these processes. A more recent meta-analysis compared motor observation, imagery, and execution (Hardwick et al., 2018) and revealed that motor execution shared more activation sites with motor imagery than with motor observation. A systematic meta-analysis comparing action language, motor observation, imagery, and execution would provide unique and important insights into the motor processes involved in action language processing.

The main objective of the present study was to provide a comprehensive analysis of the fMRI literature on action language processing and its links with other motor-related processes (motor observation, imagery, and execution) through a systematic review of the literature and a two-fold meta-analysis (ALE and ROI). The aims of the systematic review were to generate a clear portrait of research on the neural correlates of action language processing and to examine whether this literature has explored action/movement-related parameters that have been explored in other motor-related fields of research. The overall aim of the ALE meta-analysis was to characterize the motor-related network of action language processing in comparison to those of motor observation, imagery, and execution. The first specific objective was to compare action language, motor observation, and motor imagery to motor execution in order to confirm the existence of a gradation in the similarity of the neural networks associated with these processes to the motor execution network. Considering that the motor execution network is more similar to the network of motor imagery than motor observation (Hardwick et al., 2018), and that the network of action language is more similar to motor observation than execution (Rueschemeyer et al., 2014), we expected action language to be the least “motoric” of the following motor-related processes on a motor gradation: motor execution – motor imagery – motor observation – action language. The second specific objective was to determine to which motor-related process the action language network was most similar. Since the action language network has been shown to be more similar to motor observation than to motor execution (Rueschemeyer et al., 2014), we expected the degree of similarity between action language and other motor-related processes to follow the previously mentioned motor gradation: the neural network of action language would be more similar to that of motor observation, then to motor imagery and finally to motor execution. The third specific objective was to determine whether the action language network was more closely related to movement or action processing. We hypothesized that the action language network would be more similar to the action compared to the movement processing network. Finally, an anatomical ROI meta-analysis aimed to provide complementary information regarding the functional network for action language by comparing, region by region, the bilateral language activation to the profile for motor imagery, observation, and execution.

Section snippets

Search strategy

Four comprehensive electronic literature searches were performed using PubMed in January 2018 and updated in March 2019. The searches separately identified studies focusing on manual action language, motor observation, imagery, and execution. The following key search terms (in all fields) included: (1) (action language OR action verbs OR embodied language) AND (hand OR manual OR finger) AND fMRI, (2) action observation2

Meta-analysis

The general objective of the meta-analysis was to characterize the brain networks involved in action language processing and to compare these networks to those of motor observation, imagery, and execution as a way to gain further knowledge about the mechanisms involved in action language processing. The analysis was divided into two components: a series of activation likelihood estimation (ALE) analyses and an anatomical region of interest (ROI) analysis. The ALE analyses allowed for the direct

Discussion

The general objective of this study was to provide a systematic and statistically supported account of the state of research on the neural correlates of action language processing. To this aim, a systematic review of the literature and two kinds of meta-analyses were conducted (ALE and ROI-based). Four main findings emerged: (1) the motor parameters that are frequently considered in motor observation, imagery, and execution studies are rarely considered in action language research, (2) there is

Conclusion

The present research is the first to compare the functional network of action language to those of motor observation, imagery, and execution using three different approaches: a systematic review of the literature, an ALE whole-brain meta-analysis and a ROI-based meta-analysis. The review of the literature highlighted the experimental standards in research on motor observation, imagery, and execution, that action language research needs to adopt if bridges are to be built across research fields.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by a grant from the Canadian foundation for innovation to P.T. (31408), who also holds a Career Awards from the “Fonds de Recherche du Québec – Santé’’ (FRQS; 35016).

References (151)

  • R. Cunnington et al.

    The preparation and execution of self-initiated and externally-triggered movement: a study of event-related fMRI

    NeuroImage

    (2002)
  • R. Cunnington et al.

    Premovement activity of the pre-supplementary motor area and the readiness for action: studies of time-resolved event-related functional MRI

    Hum. Mov. Sci.

    (2005)
  • J. Davey

    Shared neural processes support semantic control and action understanding

    Brain Lang.

    (2015)
  • P.M. de Vries et al.

    Changed patterns of cerebral activation related to clinically normal hand movement in cervical dystonia

    Clin. Neurol. Neurosurg.

    (2008)
  • P. Dechent et al.

    Is the human primary motor cortex involved in motor imagery?

    Cogn. Brain Res.

    (2004)
  • R.H. Desai et al.

    A piece of the action: modulation of sensory-motor regions by action idioms and metaphors

    NeuroImage

    (2013)
  • I. Deschamps et al.

    On the role of the supramarginal gyrus in phonological processing and verbal working memory: evidence from rTMS studies

    Neuropsychologia

    (2014)
  • G. Di Cesare et al.

    Language for action: motor resonance during the processing of human and robotic voices

    Brain Cogn.

    (2017)
  • C. Di Dio et al.

    The neural correlates of velocity processing during the observation of a biological effector in the parietal and premotor cortex

    NeuroImage

    (2013)
  • N. El-Sourani et al.

    Making sense of objects lying around: how contextual objects shape brain activity during action observation

    NeuroImage

    (2018)
  • F. Filimon et al.

    Human cortical representations for reaching: mirror neurons for execution, observation, and imagery

    NeuroImage

    (2007)
  • F. Fiori et al.

    Enhanced action performance following TMS manipulation of associative plasticity in ventral premotor-motor pathway

    NeuroImage

    (2018)
  • A. Guillot et al.

    Functional neuroanatomical networks associated with expertise in motor imagery

    NeuroImage

    (2008)
  • P. Hagoort

    Nodes and networks in the neural architecture for language: broca’s region and beyond

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (2014)
  • R.M. Hardwick et al.

    Neural correlates of action_ comparing meta-analyses of imagery, observation, and execution

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2018)
  • U. Hasson et al.

    Grounding the neurobiology of language in first principles_ the necessity of non-language-centric explanations for language comprehension

    Cognition

    (2018)
  • O. Hauk et al.

    Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex

    Neuron

    (2004)
  • D. Jirak et al.

    Grasping language – a short story on embodiment

    Conscious. Cogn.

    (2010)
  • M. Jonas et al.

    Do simple intransitive finger movements consistently activate frontoparietal mirror neuron areas in humans?

    NeuroImage

    (2007)
  • D. Kemmerer et al.

    Neuroanatomical distribution of five semantic components of verbs: evidence from fMRI

    Brain Lang.

    (2008)
  • G. Koch et al.

    In vivo definition of parieto-motor connections involved in planning of grasping movements

    NeuroImage

    (2010)
  • K. Kudo et al.

    Selective activation and deactivation of the human brain structures between speeded and precisely timed tapping responses to identical visual stimulus: an fMRI study

    NeuroImage

    (2004)
  • R.P. Maguire et al.

    Evidence of enhancement of spatial attention during inhibition of a visuo-motor response

    NeuroImage

    (2003)
  • B.Z. Mahon et al.

    A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content

    J. Physiol. (Lond.)

    (2008)
  • T. McDowell et al.

    TMS over the supramarginal gyrus delays selection of appropriate grasp orientation during reaching and grasping tools for use

    Cortex

    (2018)
  • L. Meteyard et al.

    Coming of age: a review of embodiment and the neuroscience of semantics

    Cortex

    (2012)
  • N. Mizuguchi et al.

    Brain activity during motor imagery of an action with an object: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study

    Neurosci. Res.

    (2013)
  • N. Mizuguchi et al.

    Effector-independent brain activity during motor imagery of the upper and lower limbs: an fMRI study

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (2014)
  • C.L. Moody et al.

    Effects of implied physical effort in sensory-motor and pre-frontal cortex during language comprehension

    NeuroImage

    (2010)
  • R. Möttönen et al.

    Neural basis of understanding communicative actions: changes associated with knowing the actor’s intention and the meanings of the actions

    Neuropsychologia

    (2016)
  • D.G. Nair et al.

    Cortical and cerebellar activity of the human brain during imagined and executed unimanual and bimanual action sequences: a functional MRI study

    Cogn. Brain Res.

    (2003)
  • R. Newman-Norlund et al.

    The role of inferior frontal and parietal areas in differentiating meaningful and meaningless object-directed actions

    Brain Res.

    (2010)
  • U. Noppeney et al.

    Action selectivity in parietal and temporal cortex

    Cogn. Brain Res.

    (2005)
  • N. Picard et al.

    Imaging the premotor areas

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (2001)
  • J. Plata Bello et al.

    The mirror neuron system and motor dexterity: What happens?

    Neuroscience

    (2014)
  • C. Press et al.

    Automatic imitation of intransitive actions

    Brain Cogn.

    (2008)
  • W. Prinz

    Action representation: crosstalk between semantics and pragmatics

    Neuropsychologia

    (2014)
  • S.V. Adamovich et al.

    A virtual reality-based system integrated with fmri to study neural mechanisms of action observation-execution: a proof of concept study

    Restor. Neurol. Neurosci.

    (2009)
  • B.M. Adhikari et al.

    Enhanced brain network activity in complex movement rhythms: a simultaneous functional magnetic resonance imaging and electroencephalography study

    Brain Connect.

    (2018)
  • P. Bach et al.

    On the role of object information in action observation: an fMRI study

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2010)
  • Cited by (12)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    Present address: Département de neurosciences, Université de Montréal, Centre de Recherche de l’Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal, 4545 chemin Queen-Mary, H3W 1W4, Montreal, QC, Canada.

    View full text