Original Article
Visual Analog Scale pain reporting was standardized

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2004.11.017Get rights and content

Abstract

Background and Objective

Whereas pain is frequently measured using a visual analog scale (VAS) that can examine change over short time intervals in the same subject, such ratings are not useful in analyzing differences across subjects. We created a method for normalizing VAS pain reporting to control for the variation between different populations due to the differences in subjective perception or objective evaluation of pain.

Methods

A list of 226 pains was gathered from a convenience sample of persons on the street and patients waiting at medical and orthopedic clinics. These pains were ranked according to severity by health professionals and 19 pains with the most stable rankings were selected. These 19 pains were then rated by a sample of community-dwelling persons and a method of VAS standardization was developed, based on six selected pains.

Results

Individual variations in pain ratings were found to be independent of respondent age and gender, but were correlated with experience of the event or behavior and with self-reported health status. A new scoring method that takes into account these correlations is proposed.

Conclusion

It is possible to standardize VAS pain ratings to compare pain between different populations.

Introduction

Pain is a universal experience, but reports of pain are hard to interpret. Much work has gone into characterizing and measuring pain. Our goal is to find a way to make the reporting of pain experience more comparable across individuals in a population. We focus our attention on developing a statistical adjustment process that can be applied to a widely used pain reporting device, the Visual Analog Scale (VAS).

The phenomenon of pain includes both subjective perception and actual experience, and the combination of the two. Excellent reviews of pain measurement are available [1], [2], [3], [4]. Different subjects may report different levels of pain as a result of the same physical stimulus, based presumably on a combination of physiological as well as psychosocial factors [5], [6], [7]. Given the complex networks for receiving and interpreting pain, the same level of injury may affect different people differently [2], [3], [8]. These observations raise the question about how useful self-reported pain data are for comparisons among people reporting pain, or even within the same individual over two different times.

Pain reporting may vary according to demographic factors [1], [9], [10], [11], [12], [13], [14], [15]. Physicians respond differently to reporting of pain by patients from different ethnic groups [16]. Moreover, evidence suggests that past pain experiences influence the rating of pain severity [17].

Pain is considered to be a multidimensional construct [18], [19], and simple pain measures are viewed with skepticism [20]. Nonetheless, many clinical investigators have used some variant of the VAS to generate a quantitative measure of pain. This method appears to be reliable in tracking changes in pain over time in the same subject, but, given the evidence regarding the subjective (psychosocial) component to the rating of pain, it does not permit any comparisons of pain experienced between populations (e.g., persons with Parkinson disease versus those with multiple sclerosis).

One attempt at developing a method for standardizing VAS reports by adjusting for reports of a predetermined standard set of pains was unable to improve the performance of the underlying VAS approach [21], but the problem may lie in choosing an arbitrary set of standard pains that were not necessarily familiar to the subjects. Ideally, the approach to norming individual pain reports or pain reports between populations would use pains that are consistently felt to represent different severity levels. Although such consistency is an ideal, little is known about the actual experience of pain in the general population in terms of either its prevalence or their perception of how painful it is [22], [23], [24], [25].

The present study explored people's ratings of various pains (whether experienced or not) and the relationship of those ratings to their actual pain experience, as a step toward creating a method that would permit norming responses across respondents and thus allow comparing pain ratings on a common metric.

Section snippets

Methods

A multistage approach was used, each with a different sample. These stages are presented in Table 1. The first three stages represent the work reported here; the fourth stage uses the tool developed in this study. In the first stage, a convenience sample of 313 persons was interviewed: 104 patients in orthopaedic surgery clinic waiting rooms, 100 patients from primary care medicine clinic waiting rooms, and 109 persons stopped on the street or in a shopping mall. Each person was asked to

Results

The average rating of the severity of pain by the community sample in the third stage was found to be similar to the rank ordering by health professionals in the prior step (Table 2). The Spearman correlation coefficient between ranks was .965 (P < .001).

Although 1,622 community respondents (81%) returned the questionnaires, not all respondents answered every question. Table 3 shows their mean ratings, organized by whether they had experienced the pain. In 11 of the 19 pains, those who had

Discussion

Several observations emerge from the present study. Few pains (more often minor ones) are universally experienced by men or women, nor do people universally agree about the severity of a given pain. These findings indicate the need for a way to normalize self-reported pain using VAS items that takes into account the differences that exist between individuals regarding not just experience but the perception of how much pain is associated with an event.

It is possible to identify candidate pains

References (30)

  • R. Sternbach

    Pain and “hassles” in the United States: findings of the Nuprin pain report

    Pain

    (1986)
  • M. Zborowski

    People in pain

    (1969)
  • P.D. Wall

    Pain: the science of suffering

    (1999)
  • D.D. Price

    Psychological mechanisms of pain and analgesia

    (1999)
  • Cited by (51)

    • Endoscopic retrieval of retracted flexor tendons: An atraumatic technique

      2019, Journal of Plastic, Reconstructive and Aesthetic Surgery
      Citation Excerpt :

      The patients were asked to rate their surgical site pain after surgery with reference to pain at rest, while moving and while placing the hand on a surface. The general perceptions of the pain from common experiences were also assessed by general pain questions (pain from sprained ankle/wrist) to determine any potential differences in pain tolerance.23 Such standardisation for pain perception is necessary and suggested in other studies using VAS scoring.24

    • Comparative Morbidity of Cubital Tunnel Surgeries: A Prospective Cohort Study

      2018, Journal of Hand Surgery
      Citation Excerpt :

      Patients completed patient-reported questionnaires before surgery and then at 1 to 3 weeks, 4 to 8 weeks, and more than 8 weeks (mean, 12 weeks; range, 9–26 weeks) after surgery. Visual analog scales (VAS) quantified preoperative pain related to the cubital tunnel syndrome and also assessed general pain perception from common experiences (pain from stubbing toe, pain from a paper cut).17 The general pain questions were collected to examine for potential differences in pain tolerance between the groups because such standardization for sensory experiences has been suggested in other fields using VAS scoring.18

    • Continuous Paravertebral Infusions as an Effective Adjunct for Postoperative Pain Management in Living Liver Donors: A Retrospective Observational Study

      2017, Transplantation Proceedings
      Citation Excerpt :

      Every patient experiences acute pain during the 72 hours after invasive surgery. But because no standard scale exists, self-reported pain scores are difficult to interpret [23]. We can never be sure that self-reported pain, whether expressed verbally or as a numeric value, is comparable from one patient to the next.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    Present address: Department of Orthopaedic Surgery, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA.

    View full text