Generalizability of carry-over effects in the emotional Stroop task

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2004.06.003Get rights and content

Abstract

The emotional Stroop task has been the most widely used task to examine attentional bias to emotionally salient stimuli. In one format of this task, words are presented to participants in a mixed randomized or quasi-randomized sequence. Using a mixed smoking Stroop task, we have previously demonstrated that smokers are slower to respond to words which follow smoking-related words than words which follow neutral words. Here we show that this carry-over effect is present in heroin addicts—but not control subjects—in a heroin Stroop task, and in normal subjects in a stress Stroop task. Thus, the effect generalizes to other populations. In addition, an examination of the studies that have collected data from both mixed and blocked formats provides converging evidence for the presence of carry-over effects. We discuss the implications of the carry-over effect for research using the emotional Stroop task.

Section snippets

Study 1

Franken et al. (2000) examined attentional bias in heroin addicts (n=21, mean age=31.5) and control participants (n=33, mean age=34.8) using a mixed version of the Stroop task. The heroin addicts were abstinent inpatients from a clinical treatment center (mean time of current abstinence=9 weeks, SD=8.9). Franken et al. (2000) reported that heroin addicts, but not controls, were slower to color-name heroin-related words than neutral words in a supraliminal Stroop task.1

Study 2

Sayette et al. (2001) investigated the stress-reducing effects of alcohol on a number of stress-relevant dependent measures, including a mixed version of a stress Stroop task. Social drinkers (N=165, age range 21–28) completed the stress Stroop task after drinking alcohol or a placebo beverage. Stress was induced prior to task performance by asking participants to present a self-disclosing speech about their physical appearance. This speech was delivered immediately after completing the Stroop

Study 3

The trial-level analyses presented in Table 1, Table 2 (and those presented in Waters et al., 2003) constitute the most direct method to detect the presence of carry-over effects. An additional, indirect, method is to examine whether the pattern of data in the literature suggests the presence of carry-over effects. The most straightforward predictions can be derived from studies in which emotional Stroop performance was measured on both a blocked and mixed format in the same study. If

General discussion

We have previously reported that a carry-over effect is present in smokers using a mixed format of the smoking Stroop task (Waters et al., 2003). Here we have extended this finding in a number of ways. First, we have shown that heroin addicts, but not controls, exhibit a carry-over effect in a mixed heroin Stroop task (Study 1). Thus the carry-over effect appears robust across different types of addictions, and is sensitive to group. Second, we have shown that the effect can be observed in

Acknowledgement

This study was supported in part by a grant from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (AA09918) awarded to Michael Sayette, and by a grant to the Dutch Organization for Scientific Research (NWO).

The syntax for implementing the mixed-level modeling in SAS is available on request.

References (29)

  • I.H.A. Franken et al.

    Selective cognitive processing of drug cues in heroin dependence

    J. Psychopharmacol.

    (2000)
  • I.H.A. Franken et al.

    Neurophysiological evidence for abnormal cognitive processing of drug cues in heroin dependence

    Psychopharmacology

    (2003)
  • C.R. Harris et al.

    Attention and the processing of emotional words and names

    Psychol. Sci.

    (2004)
  • C. Holle et al.

    The effects of blocked versus random presentation and semantic relatedness of stimulus words on response to a modified Stroop task among social phobics

    Cognitive Ther. Res.

    (1997)
  • Cited by (87)

    • Chronic cannabis use and attentional bias: Extended attentional capture to cannabis cues

      2018, Addictive Behaviors
      Citation Excerpt :

      Attentional bias can be separated into engagement and disengagement processes (Posner, Inhoff, Friedrich, & Cohen, 1987). Recent studies have suggested that attentional engagement supports the development of addiction, while attentional disengagement contributes to the maintenance of addiction (van Hemel-Ruiter, de Jong, Oldehinkel, & Ostafin, 2013; Waters, Sayette, Franken, & Schwartz, 2005). In response to drug-related stimuli, attentional engagement occurs relatively early and automatically (e.g., 250 ms), while attentional disengagement occurs relatively later (e.g., 500 ms) reflecting impaired attentional shifts (van Hemel-Ruiter et al., 2013).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text