Elsevier

Biological Psychology

Volume 123, February 2017, Pages 226-234
Biological Psychology

Overprotective social support leads to increased cardiovascular and subjective stress reactivity

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.12.009Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Overprotective social support may undermine active stress-coping.

  • Overprotection did not buffer CVR during support reception.

  • Stopping overprotection induced increased cardiovascular and subjective stress.

  • Social support stress-buffering research can benefit from Self-Determination theory.

  • Autonomy support is recommended when delivering no support is no option.

Abstract

Objective

Self-determination theory suggests that autonomy-enhancing social support helps individuals to perceive stressors as challenging rather than stressing. Overprotective support may reduce stress in the short-run but undermines autonomy, thus hampering stress-coping in the long run, particularly when social support is terminated.

Method

Heartrate, blood-pressure and ratings were examined in N = 44 undergraduate students receiving autonomy support (calculation steps) or overprotection (solutions) from a close friend or no support for solving arithmetic tasks as well as during a subsequent stress-challenge (solving arithmetic tasks alone).

Results

Overprotection resulted in increased heartrate, diastolic blood-pressure, stress ratings, and decreased subjective control during stress-challenge. Autonomy support did not lead to unfavorable stress responding.

Conclusion

The current findings are in line with assumptions derived from self-determination theory and indicate that autonomy support can help to prevent stress. Overprotection does not buffer stress and is associated with increased stress when discontinued.

Introduction

The social support stress-buffering hypothesis (Cohen & Wills, 1985) suggests that social support reduces detrimental cardiovascular reactivity (CVR) in stressful situations, which is a predictor of reduced risk for developing hypertension or coronary heart disease (Treiber et al., 2003). Although numerous studies support the stress-buffering and health-promoting effect of social support, there are significant differences between effects of perceived and received support (Haber, Cohen, Lucas, & Baltes, 2007; Uchino, 2009). Perceived support describes a person’s potential access to supportive resources in everyday life, is independent of the actual reception of support (Cohen & Wills, 1985), and is associated with increased cardiovascular health and decreased cardiac and all-cause mortality (Barth, Schneider, & von Känel, 2010; Berkman, Glass, Brissette, & Seeman, 2000; Holt-Lunstad, Smith, & Layton, 2010; House, Landis, & Umberson, 1988; Shor, Roelfs, & Yogev, 2013). Received social support on the other hand refers to actual support that a person receives within a certain time frame and situation (Wills & Shinar, 2000) and has generated rather mixed results. Forster & Stoller (1992) for example found received support to be associated with an increase in mortality. These effects seem to reflect more than just the health status of a person and their need for support (Uchino, Carlisle, Birmingham, & Vaughn, 2011). A high amount of support is often perceived as overprotection and therefore a threat to one’s self-efficacy. Reinhardt, Boerner, and Horowitz (2006) argued that a high amount of instrumental support is detrimental in individuals with chronic impairment because it draws their attention to their inabilities/difficulties in daily activities. Increased stress and decreased self-efficacy/self-esteem due to received overprotective support have been found in different chronic conditions: cardiac patients (Berkhuysen, Nieuwland, Buunk, Sanderman, & Rispens, 1999; Clarke, Walker, & Cuddy, 1996; Condon & McCarthy, 2006; Coyne & Smith, 1991; Joekes, Van Elderen, & Schreurs, 2007), cancer patients (Kuijer et al., 2000; Lepore, Glaser, & Roberts, 2008), patients with disabilities (de Leon, Gold, Glass, Kaplan, & George, 2001; Dunbar, Ford, & Hunt, 1998), patients with visual impairment (Cimarolli, Reinhardt, & Horowitz, 2006; Reinhardt et al., 2006), dialysis patients (Jansen et al., 2014), and patients with multimorbidity (Warner et al., 2011).

Laboratory studies examining the social support stress-buffering hypothesis typically investigated the influence of the presence of supportive others, or the mental activation of supportive ties in a standardized stress task (e.g. public speaking). In a meta-analysis by Thorsteinsson and James (1999) received social support was related to attenuated heart rate (HR), systolic blood pressure (SBP), diastolic blood pressure (DBP), skin conductance level and cortisol levels. On the other hand some studies have failed to find a stress-buffering effect of social support (Anthony and O'Brien, 1999, Taylor et al., 2010), and some studies found increased physiological stress reactivity to social support (Gramer & Reitbauer, 2010; Hilmert, Christenfeld, & Kulik, 2002; Phillips, Gallagher, & Carroll, 2009) or presence of others (Gramer, 2002). While type and quality of the relationship to the support provider seem to have a huge impact on the stress-buffering effect of receiving social support in the laboratory (Gramer, 2002, Gramer and Supp, 2014; Uno, Uchino, & Smith, 2002), exaggerated reactivity has often shown to be a consequence of evaluation aspects of the experimental situation (Phillips et al., 2009) meaning that participants feel controlled and lose self-efficacy, which may in turn increase stress reactivity (Hodgins, Brown, & Carver, 2007).

Self-Determination-Theory (SDT) by Ryan and Deci (2000) provides an interesting approach for reconciling the seemingly contradictory effects of received social support. SDT states three basic psychological needs. These are autonomy, the feeling that one’s behavior is self-congruent and intrinsically determined; competence, the need to feel the capability of influencing the environment in ways one intends to; and relatedness, the feeling of being close to and connected with one’s social environment. These basic needs are considered critical for intrinsic motivation (Teixeira, Carraca, Markland, Silva, & Ryan, 2012), coping with stress (Ntoumanis, Edmunds, & Duda, 2009) and psychological well-being (Milyavskaya & Koestner, 2011). Viewing the social support stress-buffering hypothesis from a SDT perspective, receiving support should be most helpful when it fosters one’s basic needs. Accordingly, the supported person should perceive themselves as autonomous, should feel competent to cope with the stressor, and should feel related to the person or environment that provides the support. In an autonomy-supporting environment individuals tend to experience stressful situations as challenging rather than threatening and, in turn, cope more actively with them (Weinstein & Ryan, 2011). Typically, an autonomy supporting environment also responds to other basic needs and individuals feeling autonomous in their behavior tend to perceive themselves as more competent and related (Baard, Deci, & Ryan, 2004). This can increase self-efficacy in coping with stressful situations (Hodgins et al., 2007). An overprotective support style on the other hand may undermine the individual’s basic needs, induce feelings of being controlled and evaluated (Bartholomew, Ntoumanis, Ryan, Bosch, & Thøgersen-Ntoumani, 2011) and may therefore undermine an individual’s subjective competence and self-efficacy (Joekes, Van Elderen, & Schreurs, 2007). Receiving “too much” support may therefore be an additional stressor rather than a stress-buffer.

To our knowledge, not many studies have investigated the relationship of autonomy support on physiological parameters. In a study by Hodgins et al. (2010) participants showed improved task performance and attenuated cardiovascular response after motivational priming towards autonomous rather than controlled motivation. In a more recent study of Weinstein et al. (2016), couples underwent a laboratory conflict conversation and participants that rated their partner as more autonomy supporting in general, showed lower DBP activation after the conversation. Social support studies that shed light on the important role of autonomy according to the stress-buffering hypothesis are studies investigating “invisible support” – a support style that is characterized as non-evaluative and non-directive because the supported individual is not aware of the support act (Bolger and Amarel, 2007, Kirsch and Lehman, 2014). In these studies “invisibly supported” participants showed decreased SBP and DBP to a speech task compared to participants that received directive support for the task. The authors argue that this style of support can reduce feelings of being evaluated, which may reduce related negative emotions and enhance realizing one’s coping resources (Howland & Simpson, 2010). While this research sheds light on the importance of preserving an individual’s autonomy during support interactions, up to date no study investigated how receiving overprotective support affects CVR and whether receiving autonomy support can foster individual’s feelings of being in control with regards to stressors in the long-term.

Based on this previous research we would expect overprotective social support to increase stress rather than buffering it, because individuals can be expected to feel evaluated and threatened in their self-esteem. Additionally, we would expect autonomy support not to be a direct stress-buffer, but rather to have a beneficial effect on the long-term. If an autonomy supporting environment fosters an individual’s self-efficacy and control experience in coping with a stressor, individuals should show decreased CVR also when facing a stressor alone. In contrast overprotection may rather undermine one’s competence and self-efficacy, leading to increased stress when the need arises to face a stressor without the (overly) comforting support.

Therefore the current experiment aimed at investigating effects of autonomy support vs. overprotection from a close friend on physiological reactivity and stress appraisal in a four-phase laboratory stress task: A practice phase, in which participants got used to the task setting, a learning phase in which participants “learned” to cope with a stressor within a specific supportive type available, a stress-challenge phase in which participants had to cope with the stressor without support, and a recovery phase without activity. We expect that positive effects of autonomy support would particularly affect the stress-challenge phase because these participants were expected to perceive themselves as strengthened in their own competence of facing a stressor alone. In contrast the overprotected participants were expected to be particularly negatively affected by the threat of facing a stressor alone. This view is in line with other authors that have suggested that effects of supportive interactions are more likely to be found delayed in general (e.g. Gramer & Supp, 2014). Therefore we expected (1) that autonomy support should decrease physiological reactivity in the stress-challenge phase, while overprotective support should increase it. We also expected that (2) overprotective support should increase appraisal of stress and decrease appraisal of control in the stress-challenge phase.

Section snippets

Participants

Fifty-two undergraduate students were recruited from the student pool of the University of Würzburg, Germany who received course credits for study participation. Exclusion criteria included acute or chronic cardiovascular disease and being on current cardiovascular medication. Four participants were excluded from the analysis because they stated at the end of the experimental procedure in an open-ended question that they were suspicious of the manipulation (see “Procedure”); four participants

Sample characteristics and group differences

One-way ANOVAs revealed no significant differences between the three experimental conditions (autonomy support, overprotection, and no support) for socio-demographic (age, BMI, math grade, nicotine, and stimulant beverage consumption) or any baseline measure of the dependent physiologic variables (consumption of stimulant beverages, F(2.23.53) = 3.25, p= 0.06, η2partial = 0.18, all other p’s > 0.48; see Table 1). Although the report of daily consumed stimulant beverages differed marginally between the

Discussion

The present study investigated the effects of social support, in particular, autonomy support vs. overprotection, on physiological and subjective stress reactivity. The results show that these two support types mainly differ when individuals have to cope with the stressor on their own after having received a particular type of support (stress-challenge phase, i.e. solving mental arithmetic without social support), not while getting the support (learning phase). While none of the support types

Conclusion

In sum, SDT provided a valuable theoretic guideline for conducting the present study examining the social support stress-buffering hypothesis from a novel perspective.

The present findings are a first indicator that overprotection did not buffer stress-responding in the short run (i.e., while it is received) and may have rather detrimental effects in the long-run, because it was associated with increased physiological and subjective stress reactivity once it was terminated. In contrast, autonomy

Acknowledgements

During preparation of this manuscript SMS and RZ were supported by the Comprehensive Heart Failure Center (CHFC), University Hospital Würzburg, Germany, through project 01EO1004 funded by the Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF). RZ and SMS designed the study; RZ collected and analyzed the data and prepared the manuscript; SMS and PP revised the manuscript critically. All authors have approved of the final version. We kindly thank Domenika Kusch, Johanna Döbig, Tobias Frosch and

References (72)

  • R. Amthauer et al.

    Intelligenz-Struktur-Test 2000 r [Intelligence structure test 2000 r (I-S-T 2000 R)]

    (2001)
  • J.L. Anthony et al.

    An evaluation of the impact of social support manipulations on cardiovascular reactivity to laboratory stressors

    Behavioral Medicine

    (1999)
  • P.P. Baard et al.

    Intrinsic need satisfaction: A motivational basis of performance and weil-being in two work settings1

    Journal of Applied Social Psychology

    (2004)
  • S.R. Baker et al.

    Uncertainty of outcomes as a component of active coping: Influence of predictability and feedback on heart rate reactivity and task performance

    Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2000)
  • J. Barth et al.

    Lack of social support in the etiology and the prognosis of Coronary heart disease: A systematic review and meta-analysis

    Psychosomatic Medicine

    (2010)
  • K.J. Bartholomew et al.

    Self-determination theory and diminished functioning: The role of interpersonal control and psychological need thwarting

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (2011)
  • N. Bolger et al.

    Effects of social support visibility on adjustment to stress experimental evidence

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2007)
  • M. Carlisle et al.

    Subliminal activation of social ties moderates cardiovascular reactivity during acute stress

    Health Psychology

    (2012)
  • N. Christenfeld et al.

    Social support effects on cardiovascular reactivity: Is a stranger as effective as a friend?

    Psychosomatic Medicine

    (1997)
  • V.R. Cimarolli et al.

    Perceived overprotection: Support gone bad?

    The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences

    (2006)
  • D.E. Clarke et al.

    The role of perceived overprotectiveness in recovery 3 months after myocardial infarction

    Journal of Cardiopulmonary Rehabilitation

    (1996)
  • S. Cohen et al.

    Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1985)
  • C. Condon et al.

    Lifestyle changes following acute myocardial infarction: Patients perspectives

    European Journal of Cardiovascular Nursing

    (2006)
  • J.C. Coyne et al.

    Couples coping with a myocardial infarction: A contextual perspective on wives' distress

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1991)
  • F.W. Craig et al.

    Can male-provided social support buffer the cardiovascular responsivity to stress in men? It depends on the nature of the support provided

    International Journal of Men’s Health

    (2002)
  • C.E. Cutrona et al.

    Type of social support and specific stress: Towards a theory of optimal matching

  • C.F.M. de Leon et al.

    Disability as a function of social networks and support in elderly african americans and whites: The duke EPESE 1986–1992

    The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences

    (2001)
  • M. Dunbar et al.

    Why is the receipt of social support associated with increased psychological distress? An examination of three hypotheses

    Psychology & Health

    (1998)
  • L.E. Forster et al.

    The impact of social support on mortality: A seven-year follow-up of older men and women

    Journal of Applied Gerontology

    (1992)
  • S.S. Franklin et al.

    Does the relation of blood pressure to Coronary heart disease risk change with aging?: The framingham heart study

    Circulation

    (2001)
  • W. Gerin et al.

    Self-efficacy as a moderator of perceived control effects on cardiovascular reactivity: Is enhanced control always beneficial?

    Psychosomatic Medicine

    (1995)
  • M. Gramer

    The effect of social support on cardiovascular responses to psychological challenge: Moderating influences of gender and situational variables. [Der Effekt sozialer Unterstützung auf die kardiovaskuläre Reaktivität in psychischen Belastungssituationen: moderierende Einflüsse des Geschlechts und situativer Variablen]

    Psychologische Beiträge

    (2002)
  • S.W. Greenhouse et al.

    On methods in the analysis of profile data

    Psychometrika

    (1959)
  • M.G. Haber et al.

    The relationship between self-reported received and perceived social support: A meta-analytic review

    American Journal Of Community Psychology

    (2007)
  • C.J. Hilmert et al.

    Audience status moderates the effects of social support and self-efficacy on cardiovascular reactivity during public speaking

    Annals of Behavioral Medicine

    (2002)
  • H.S. Hodgins et al.

    Autonomy and control motivation and self-esteem

    Self and Identity

    (2007)
  • Cited by (13)

    • Effects of parental care and overprotection on adolescents' diurnal cortisol profiles

      2022, Hormones and Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      Parents' denial of autonomy and excessive protection (e.g., over involvement in problem solving) may be perceived as intrusive and signal parents' distrust in adolescents' capacities to manage stress, which may frustrate their basic psychological needs for autonomy (i.e., experiencing pressure and coercion) and competence (i.e., feeling inadequacy and failure) (Van Petegem et al., 2020). Stress associated with recurrent overprotection and interference with the development of autonomy likely influences a series of stress-related psychobiological processes, such as increased perceived stress, worry, and frustration, low self-esteem and perceived control, and alterations in stress-physiology pathways (Spada et al., 2012; Van Petegem et al., 2020; Zniva et al., 2017). Analyses also revealed that parental care was associated with higher cortisol levels at awakening, while parental overprotection was associated with lower levels of cortisol at awakening.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text