Elsevier

Behavioural Brain Research

Volume 350, 17 September 2018, Pages 80-86
Behavioural Brain Research

Research report
Individual behavioural predictors of amphetamine-induced emission of 50 kHz vocalization in rats

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2018.05.009Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Motivation (latency to approach reward) and tendency to emit USVs were studied.

  • Approach latency and 50 kHz USVs after saline predict AMPH-induced USV emission.

  • Approach latency and saline 50 kHz USVs represent distinct predictors for AMPH USVs.

  • Without sensitization, AMPH selectively increased Trill subtype of USVs.

  • Acute AMPH-induced 50 kHz USVs show elevated average sound frequency.

Abstract

Measurement of ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs) produced by adult rats represents a highly useful index of emotional arousal. The associations found between 50 kHz USV production and a variety of behavioural and pharmacological protocols increasingly suggests they serve as a marker of positive motivational states. This study used a powerful within-subjects design to investigate the relationships among individual differences in approach to a sweet-food reward, predisposition to emit 50 kHz USVs spontaneously, and 50 kHz USVs emission following acute systemic administration of amphetamine. Both approach motivation and predisposition to call were found to not correlate with each other but did predict 50 kHz USV response to acute amphetamine. These two behavioural phenotypes appear to represent dissociable predictors of acute amphetamine-induced emission of 50 kHz USVs in a non-sensitization paradigm. In contrast to that, a measure of sucrose preference was not found to predict 50 kHz USV emission following amphetamine. Acute amphetamine was also found to increase average sound frequency of emitted USVs and selectively increase the proportion of Trill subtype 50 kHz USVs. Together, these data demonstrate that acute amphetamine-induced 50 kHz USVs in the adult rat represent more than just a univariate motivational state and may represent the product of dissociable subsystems of emotional behavior.

Introduction

Adult rats emit vocalizations in the ultrasonic frequency range that serve to convey their motivational and emotional state to other conspecifics [1,2]. This has made the detection and measurement of these ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs) a highly useful index of emotionality in a wide variety of experimental settings [3]. The recorded emission of so-called 50 kHz USVs provides a quantifiable metric of positive emotional states that does not require conditioning [1,2]. This call type is elicited by appetitive, positive behavioural situations [4,5], rewarding brain stimulation [6,7], and administration of psychostimulants, e.g., amphetamines [8,9]. Due to the unconditioned nature and generalizable characteristics of this form of behavior, recording of 50 kHz USVs has been utilized in many models of drug addiction and sensitization [[8], [9], [10], [11], [12], [13]]. The emission of 50 kHz USVs is generally dependent on the activity of the mesolimbic dopamine system [7,14], and associated with increased dopamine activity in the nucleus accumbens (NAc; [[15], [16], [17]]).

50 kHz USVs are categorized and characterized by their respective acoustic parameters, with the degree of sound frequency modulation appearing to be the best index of positive emotional arousal [1,2,8,18,19]. Due to differing degrees of frequency modulation observed across individual calls, 50 kHz USVs can be subdivided into ‘flat’ (non-modulated) and frequency-modulated (FM) call subtypes. These call subtypes are characterized by different sonographic profiles though they share the same average peak frequency range around 50–55 kHz [1,2,18]. These call subtypes are associated with distinct behaviours and contexts, with FM calls most strongly related to reward-associated contexts such as psychostimulant administration [5,[20], [21], [22]]. It has been suggested that 50 kHz USVs may represent motivational markers whereby they are elicited in association with the expectation/anticipation of a reward [6,23,24]. This is supported by findings of anticipation-related 50 kHz USV emission to psychostimulants which directly stimulate dopamine release in the NAc [11,22,25,26]. Moreover, beyond drugs that directly stimulate the mesolimbic dopamine system, there is evidence of non-DAergic agents (e.g. morphine and sucrose) having the capacity to elicit anticipatory or conditioned 50 kHz USVs [9,11,27].

Two conceptual components related to an animal’s response to a given reward are the hedonic value (the ‘liking’) and the motivation to consummate (the ‘wanting’) [28,29]. In the present experiment, an L-maze apparatus was used. The L-maze removes much of the aspect of reward learning typically associated with T-maze performance [30]. Evidence from rodents in the T-maze indicates that differences in dopamine levels result primarily in differences in the motivation to obtain reward [[31], [32], [33]]. Thus, a measure of latency in the L-maze should index an individual rats’ motivation to approach and consume a food reward and therefore should strongly positively correlate with individual differences in 50 kHz USV production.

The consumption of sucrose has been used widely as a measure of hedonia in rats [34,35] and appears dependent on the function of dopamine in the NAc [36,37]. Sucrose solutions, that are readily self-administered and consumed, induce significant 50 kHz USV production though without the same sensitization effect noted with cocaine self-administration [11]. Positive relationships are found between individual differences in sucrose intake and psychostimulant self-administration (cocaine and Damphetamine; AMPH) [[38], [39], [40]]. Moreover, there have been findings of positive relationships between individual differences in the preference for sucrose and high 50 kHz USV emission behavioural phenotype [35,41,42]. Following selective breeding based on 50 kHz USV emission, the high-line rats (which more readily emit 50 kHz calls) showed evidence of greater sucrose preference compared with random-line rats [42]. Thus, a measure of sucrose preference should index a rats’ individual hedonic response and should positively correlate with individual differences in 50 kHz USV production, providing some accounting for the ‘liking’ aspect of reward.

Amphetamine (AMPH) has been extensively used as a psychostimulant inducer of 50 kHz USVs with strong individual differences in response to systemic administration both in regard to call rate (‘low callers’ versus ‘high callers’) as well as subtypes emitted (‘call profile’) [22,[43], [44], [45]]. Additionally, Engelhardt et al. [46] recently found evidence of a positive relationship between spontaneous and AMPH-induced 50 kHz call emission. Moreover, these inter-individual differences in baseline 50 kHz USV production were potentiated by injections of AMPH and appeared related to trait-like differences in approach behaviour to 50 kHz USV playback. It should also be noted that inter- and intra-individual differences in calling extend beyond AMPH-induction and are found when 50 kHz USVs are elicited via non-pharmacological methods [47]. High caller rats that produce more 50 kHz USVs after systemic AMPH administration also show conditioned place preference to AMPH and a greater proportion of FM calls after AMPH when compared to low callers or controls [22,45]. In contrast, it has been reported by several researchers that locomotor activity and emission of 50 kHz USVs appear to be dissociable behavioural phenotypes with only partial overlap [9,12,22,45,46]. High caller rats that show behavioural sensitization to repeated AMPH injections in their production of 50 kHz USVs do not necessarily show locomotor sensitization of a comparable nature [22,45]. A similar dissociation between reward related behaviours is observed between 50 kHz USV emission after AMPH and social play behaviour, whereby the individual behavioural phenotypes for each behaviour are not necessarily positively correlated [48,49]. These results indicate that multiple subsystems likely underlie an individual animal’s orientation to rewards and raise the question of what AMPH-induced 50 kHz USV emission represent.

The present study set out to determine if the aspects of reward (liking and wanting) and USV production (individual predisposition to call) could be dissociated in a non-sensitization model of acute AMPH induction of 50 kHz calling. To date there appears to be a strong convergence of evidence indicating 50 kHz USVs as motivational markers indexing the ‘wanting’ component of reward [6,11,22,25]. This focus on the motivational aspect of 50 kHz USVs is especially the case in regards to research involving drugs of abuse [8,26]. However, there are discrepancies in this narrowed notion of 50 kHz USVs as signals of individual motivation phenotype as they are often dissociable from other reward-related behavioural phenotypes [13,49]. Additionally, most research that has recently found a dissociation of AMPH-induced calling behaviour and reward-related behavioural phenotypes has employed repeated administration protocols of sensitization [9,44,45]. To uncover the nature of what AMPH-induced 50 kHz USVs represent in regards to individual characteristics associated with reward and calling, the present study uses a non-sensitized, non-anticipatory AMPH-induction protocol and a within-subjects design. For each animal measures of hedonic drive (sucrose preference), motivation for reward (latency to approach and consume a food reward in an L-maze), and predisposition to emit 50 kHz USVs (calls after saline) were used to predict call rate after 1.5 mg/kg of systemic AMPH. It was hypothesized that call rate after AMPH would reflect a measure of an individual’s motivation to approach reward, their hedonic drive, and also their predisposition to emit 50 kHz USVs. These AMPH-induced 50 kHz USVs thus may represent a general positive emotional state rather than simply motivational drive. Therefore, it was hypothesized that each of these variables would predict USV response after AMPH in a dissociable fashion (see Fig. 1).

Section snippets

Subjects

Forty-six male Long Evans rats (Charles River Laboratories, Saint-Constant, QC, Canada) were used for all behavioural procedures. All animals were approximately 53 (±1) days old at the beginning of the study with an average body weight of 288 g (SD = 26.1 g, min. = 234 g, max. = 344 g). At the end of the study animals had an average body weight of 390 g (SD = 42.1 g, min. = 315 g, max. = 474 g) and were approximately 78 (±1) days old. In accordance with Brock University protocols for laboratory

Descriptive statistics

Means and standard deviations of all continuous variables (with pre-transformation data) can be found in Table 1. Subject weight (measured in grams) refers to the average weight of the rat across the duration of all behavioural procedures. Sucrose Preference refers to the individual rat average across both testing days. L-maze Latency refers to the total number of seconds across all testing sessions for a rat to approach and consume an appetitive food stimulus. Average number of calls per

Discussion

In this study, we employed a model of acute AMPH administration to induce 50 kHz calling in Long Evans rats. It was found that individual motivation to consume food reward and baseline predisposition to emit calls represent distinct predictors of the AMPH-induced call response. This finding supports the hypothesis that emission of 50 kHz USVs in response to acute administration of AMPH in the adult Long Evans rat indexes more than their individual characteristics associated with motivation for

Conclusions

The current study found evidence that approach latency to reward and emission of 50 kHz USVs after saline are unique behavioural predictors of the 50 kHz USV emission observed after acute non-anticipated systemic AMPH. This acute AMPH was found to selectively alter acoustic parameters of 50 kHz USVs by elevating average sound frequency without affecting the duration of calls. AMPH also selectively increased the proportion of Trill FM USVs, while not significantly affecting the proportion of

Competing interests

We have no competing interests to declare.

Acknowledgements and source of funding

The experimental work was supported by a Research Discovery Grant from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada to S.M.B. This study represents a fragment of doctoral dissertation of K.G.M.

References (54)

  • N. Simola et al.

    Repeated amphetamine administration and long-term effects on 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations: possible relevance to the motivational and dopamine-stimulating properties of the drug

    Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol.

    (2015)
  • N. Simola et al.

    Pharmacological characterization of 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in rats: comparison of the effects of different psychoactive drugs and relevance in drug-induced reward

    Neuropharmacology

    (2012)
  • K.C. Berridge et al.

    Neuroscience of affect: brain mechanisms of pleasure and displeasure

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (2013)
  • A. Sclafani et al.

    Hedonic response of rats to polysaccharide and sugar solutions

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (1987)
  • S. Hsiao et al.

    Raclopride reduces sucrose preference in rats

    Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav.

    (1995)
  • T. Mällo et al.

    Tickling-induced 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalization is individually stable and predicts behaviour in tests of anxiety and depression in rats

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2007)
  • E. Taracha et al.

    Diverging frequency-modulated 50-kHz vocalization, locomotor activity and conditioned place preference effects in rats given repeated amphetamine treatment

    Neuropharmacology

    (2014)
  • R.K. Schwarting et al.

    Situational factors, conditions and individual variables which can determine ultrasonic vocalizations in male adult Wistar rats

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2007)
  • C. Natusch et al.

    Using bedding in a test environment critically affects 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in laboratory rats

    Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav.

    (2010)
  • J. Burgdorf et al.

    Rats selectively bred for low levels of play-induced 50 kHz vocalizations as a model for autism spectrum disorders: a role for NMDA receptors

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2013)
  • S.M. Brudzynski et al.

    Effects of intraaccumbens amphetamine on production of 50 kHz vocalizations in three lines of selectively bred Long-Evans rats

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2011)
  • S.M. Brudzynski

    Communication of adult rats by ultrasonic vocalization: biological, sociobiological, and neuroscience approaches

    ILAR J.

    (2009)
  • M. Wöhr et al.

    Affective communication in rodents: ultrasonic vocalizations as a tool for research on emotion and motivation

    Cell Tissue Res.

    (2013)
  • J. Burgdorf et al.

    Ultrasonic vocalizations of rats (Rattus norvegicus) during mating, play, and aggression: behavioral concomitants, relationship to reward, and self-administration of playback

    J. Comp. Psychol.

    (2008)
  • J. Burgdorf et al.

    Anticipation of rewarding electrical brain stimulation evokes ultrasonic vocalization in rats

    Behav. Neurosci.

    (2000)
  • T. Scardochio et al.

    The effects of electrical and optical stimulation of midbrain dopaminergic neurons on rat 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations

    Front. Behav. Neurosci.

    (2015)
  • N. Simola et al.

    Direct and long-lasting effects elicited by repeated drug administration on 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations are regulated differently: implications for the study of the affective properties of drugs of abuse

    Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol.

    (2014)
  • Cited by (16)

    • Lesions of the rat basolateral amygdala reduce the behavioral response to ultrasonic vocalizations

      2020, Behavioural Brain Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Rodents emit calls in the ultrasonic range to express affect and transmit situation-specific information. In the rat, these ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs) can be categorized into three main classes: 50-kHz USVs are emitted during social play [1,2], after administration of amphetamine [3]and mating or tickling [4], thus in appetitive states. By contrast, 22-kHz USVs are emitted in more aversive situations, such as depression-like states [5], exposure to a predator [6] and fear learning [7].

    • Ultrasonic vocalizations as a tool in studying emotional states in rodent models of social behavior and brain disease

      2019, Neuropharmacology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Emission of USVs may provide information on the presence of positive or negative emotional states, and on the intensity of emotional states (Brudzynski, 2005; Granon et al., 2018). Besides, evidence is accumulating to suggest that measuring the emission of USV subtypes and the acoustic structure of calls may reveal how individuals differ in emotional traits, as well as in emotional responses to environmental and/or pharmacological stimuli (Chabout et al., 2012; Faure et al., 2018; Mulvihill and Brudzynski, 2018; Simola and Costa, 2018). Finally, measuring USVs may be associated with in depth analysis of meaningful behavioral data.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text