Neural network simulations of the primate oculomotor system. II. Frames of reference

https://doi.org/10.1016/0361-9230(96)00124-4Get rights and content

Abstract

Theories of motor control often assume that the location of visual stimuli is expressed in non retinotopic frames of reference. The saccadic system is known in enough detail for us to examine the evidential basis of this assumption. The organization of the neural circuit that controls saccades is first summarized. It is shown to consist of at least two interconnected modules. The first one is the burst generator, which resides in the reticular formation, and is entrusted with the tasks of impedance matching, synergist coactivation and reciprocal inhibition between antagonists. The second is a metric computer, which resides in the superior colliculus and the cerebral cortex, and computes the size and direction of the desired movement. Alternative models of the burst generator are presented and their “verisimilitude” is assessed in the light of evidence concerning saccadic trajectories, neuronal discharge patterns, interneuronal connections, as well as the results of lesion and stimulation experiments. Several models of the “metric computer” in the superior colliculus are then examined; their performance is again evaluated in the light of psychophysical, anatomical, physiological, and clinical evidence. It is demonstrated that the location of visual stimuli need not be expressed in nonretinotopic frames of reference for either the burst generator or the metric computer to issue appropriate commands to move the eyes. Instead, using information concerning intervening movements of the eyes to update the location of visual stimuli in a retinotopic frame of reference suffices for the planning and execution of correct saccades. More generally, it is proposed that the location of sensory stimuli need not be expressed in higher order frames of reference (e.g., centered in the body or even in extrapersonal space) provided that their location in a sensorium specific map is updated on the basis of effector movements.

References (55)

  • B. Biguer et al.

    The coordination of eye, head and arm movements during reaching at a single visual target

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (1982)
  • P. Dassonville et al.

    Oculomotor localization relies on a damped representation of saccadic eye displacement in human and nonhuman primates

    Vis. Neurosci.

    (1992)
  • J. Droulez et al.

    A neural network model of sensoritopic maps with predictive short-term memory properties

  • M. Flanders et al.

    Early stages in a sensorinotor transformation

    Behav. Brain Sci.

    (1992)
  • C. Galletti et al.

    Parietal neurons encoding spatial locations in craniotopic coordinates

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (1993)
  • J. Gordon et al.

    In reaching, the task is to move the hand to a target

    Behav. Brain Sci.

    (1992)
  • Grantyn, A. A.; Dalezios, Y.; Kitama, T.; Moschovakis, A. K. Neuronal mechanisms of two-dimensional orienting movements...
  • C.R.S. Kaneko

    Tests of two models of the neural saccade generator: Saccadic eye movement deficits following ibotenic acid lesions of the nuclei raphe interpositus and prepositus hypoglossi in monkey

  • C.R.S. Kaneko et al.

    Saccadic eye movement deficits following ibotenic acid lesions of the nuclei raphe interpositus and prepositus hypoglossi in monkey

    Acta Otolaryngol. (Stockh.)

    (1991)
  • W.M. King et al.

    Reticular control of vertical saccadic eye movements by mesencephalic burst neurons

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (1979)
  • T. Kokkoroyannis et al.

    The efferent projections of the intestitial nucleus of Cajal in the squirrel monkey

    Eur. J. Neurosci.

    (1995)
  • M. Kuperstein

    Neural model of adaptive hand-eye coordination for simple postures

    Science

    (1988)
  • R. Lal et al.

    Gating of retinal transmission by afferent eye position and movement signals

    Science

    (1989)
  • D.J. Maxwell et al.

    Ultrastructure of hair follicle afferent terminations in the spinal cord of the cat

    J. Neurocytol.

    (1982)
  • D.J. Maxwell et al.

    Fine structure of primary axon terminals projecting from rapidly adapting mechanoreceptors of the toe and foot pads of the cat

    Q. J. Exp. Physiol.

    (1984)
  • A. McKenzie et al.

    Properties of signals that determine the amplitude and direction of saccadic eye movements in monkeys

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (1986)
  • P. Morasso

    Spatial control of arm movements

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (1981)
  • Cited by (17)

    • Implications of interrupted eye-head gaze shifts for resettable integrator reset

      2006, Brain Research Bulletin
      Citation Excerpt :

      Although not tested in circumstances identical to the herein simulated ones (i.e., when the line of sight shifts towards a distractor before its redirection to the “target”), this is true even when the line of sight shifts in the interval between presentation of a target and execution of a saccade towards it (e.g., “double-step” stimulation experiments [12]). The neural processes needed to account for the accuracy of saccades in double-step stimulation experiments are thought to be complete by the time commands exit the SC (reviewed in [43,26]) and are therefore beyond the scope of the present model. Suffices to say that a model that is consistent with subject performance and relies on signals indicative of eye displacement rather than eye position has been proposed for the SC in the form of the “vector subtraction hypothesis” [28,26].

    • The superior colliculus and eye movement control

      1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology
    • Web alert Neural control

      1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text