Elsevier

Hormones and Behavior

Volume 63, Issue 1, January 2013, Pages 40-48
Hormones and Behavior

Influence of ovarian and non-ovarian estrogens on weight gain in response to disruption of sweet taste — calorie relations in female rats

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.11.003Get rights and content

Abstract

Regulation of energy balance in female rats is known to differ along a number of dimensions compared to male rats. Previous work from our lab has demonstrated that in female rats fed dietary supplements containing high-intensity sweeteners that may disrupt a predictive relation between sweet tastes and calories, excess weight gain is demonstrated only when females are also fed a diet high in fat and sugar, and is evidenced primarily in animals already prone to gain excess weight. In contrast, male rats show excess weight gain when fed saccharin-sweetened yogurt supplements when fed both standard chow diets and diets high in fat and sugar, and regardless of their proneness to excess weight gain. The goal of the present experiments was to determine whether ovarian, or other sources of estrogens, contributes to the resistance to excess weight gain in female rats fed standard chow diets along with dietary supplements sweetened with yogurt. Results of the first experiment indicated that when the ovaries were removed surgically in adult female rats, patterns of weight gain were similar in animals fed saccharin-sweetened compared to glucose-sweetened yogurt supplements. In the second experiment, when the ovaries were surgically removed in adult female rats, and local production of estrogens was suppressed with the aromatase inhibitor anastrozole, females fed the saccharin-sweetened yogurt consumed more energy and gained more weight than females fed the glucose-sweetened yogurt. However, when the ovaries were surgically removed prior to the onset of puberty (at 24–25 days of age), females given saccharin-sweetened yogurt along with vehicle gained excess weight. In contrast, weight gain was similar in those given saccharin-sweetened and glucose-sweetened yogurt along with anastrozole. The results suggest that behavioral differences between males and females in response to disruption of sweet→calorie relations may result from differences in patterns of local estrogen production. These differences may be established developmentally during the pubertal period in females.

Highlights

► Female rats ovariectomized (OVX) as adults do not gain excess weight when given saccharin-sweetened foods. ► Females OVX as adolescents show excess weight gain when given saccharin. ► Females OVX as adults given saccharin show excess weight gain when local production of estrogen is suppressed. ► Local production of estrogens may affect energy regulation in female rats. ► Organizational changes in energy balance may occur during puberty in females.

Introduction

Overweight and obesity represent increasingly pressing health problems in the United States and throughout the world (e.g., Flegal et al., 2012, James, 2008, Ogden et al., 2012). Efforts to reduce the spread of obesity have focused on a variety of factors, including changing the food environment to reduce energy intake. One method promoted for reducing energy intake is to replace ingredients in the diet that provide energy, such as sugars and fats, with substitutes, such as the high intensity sweeteners saccharin, acesulfame potassium (AceK) and sucralose and the fat substitute olestra, which mimic the sensory properties of sugars and fats (American Dietetic Association, 2004). The rationale behind such substitution approaches is that these replacers will result in reduced energy intake. However, whether such reductions in energy intake actually occur remains an open question. For example, a number of epidemiological studies link consumption of beverages sweetened with high-intensity sweeteners to increased risk of overweight, obesity, diabetes, cardiovascular disease and metabolic syndrome (e.g., Dhingra et al., 2007, Duffey et al., 2012, Fowler et al., 2008, Gardener et al., 2012, Laska et al., 2012, Ludwig, 2009, Lutsey et al., 2008, Nettleton et al., 2009, Yang, 2010) while other studies do not appear to demonstrate such a link (e.g., Bellisle and Drewnowski, 2007, Schulze et al., 2004).

Previously we reported that male rats given high intensity sweeteners (i.e., saccharin, acesulfame potassium, and Stevia extracts) exhibit increased energy intake and body weight gain (for review, see Swithers et al., 2010). We have provided evidence that these deficits in regulating energy balance are based on the disruption of the learned signaling relationship between sweet tastes and caloric or energetic outcomes (Davidson et al., 2011). This relationship is formed early in life (Swithers et al., 2012) presumably as a result of normal experience with consuming sweet-tasting, caloric foods and fluids.

This learned relation between taste and calories may provide one mechanism that animals use to regulate energy intake. Based on the principles of Pavlovian conditioning, as a consequence of experience with sweet tastes in foods that provide energy in the form of calories, may come to show conditioned, anticipatory responses that prepare them to effectively utilize that energy (e.g., Woods and Ramsay, 2000). Within this framework, consuming high-intensity sweeteners that taste sweet but deliver few or no calories, should interfere with energy regulation by weakening this predictive relationship (e.g., Davidson and Swithers, 2004, Swithers and Davidson, 2008). This mechanism may contribute to some of the associations observed in humans between consumption of foods and beverages sweetened with high-intensity sweeteners and increased risk of negative health outcomes including overweight, obesity, and metabolic syndrome.

To date, most of the work examining the consequences of disrupting sweet taste-calorie associations on long-term food intake and body weight gain has focused on lean male rats, although data suggest that the most likely consumers of such “diet” foods and beverages are women and those already overweight or obese (e.g., Duffey et al., 2012, Fowler et al., 2008). Recent data from our lab suggest that negative effects of consuming high-intensity sweeteners on weight control may be greatest for these overweight or likely to become overweight. We found that female rats prone to excess weight gain and fed a diet high in fat and sugar, show increased body weight when given dietary supplements sweetened with saccharin compared to those given supplements sweetened with glucose (Swithers et al., submitted for publication). However, compared to male rats, female rats consuming a standard low-fat lab chow diet appear to be relatively resistant to the additional body weight gain related to consumption of high-intensity sweeteners; that is female rats given saccharin-sweetened diets gain similar amounts as female rats given glucose-sweetened diets (Swithers et al., submitted for publication). Further, the responses of female rats to high-intensity sweeteners when maintained on diets high in fat and sugar are modulated by their phenotype; females identified as prone to diet-induced obesity (DIO) prior to introduction of the sweeteners show excess weight gain compared to those given a caloric sweetener, while females identified as resistant to diet-induced obesity (DR) gain similar amounts of weight when given caloric and non-caloric sweeteners. In contrast, both DIO and DR males show excess weight gain when provided with high-intensity sweeteners compared to caloric sweeteners (Swithers et al., submitted for publication).

The mechanisms that might underlie differences in patterns of responding to high-intensity sweeteners, and the resistance to weight gain on a standard chow diet, in female compared to male rats are unknown. However, short-term data from rats tested during the pre-pubertal period suggest that prior to the onset of puberty (e.g. 15–25 days of age), responses to high-intensity sweeteners are similar in male and female rats fed a standard chow diet (e.g., Davidson and Swithers, 2004, Swithers et al., 2012), suggesting that changes occurring during puberty may alter responses of female rats to high-intensity sweeteners. One implication of this finding is that ovarian estrogens might play a role in differences in responses of female rats before puberty compared to following puberty. Alternatively, it is becoming increasingly recognized that physiological and neural function can be influenced not only by circulating estrogens derived from the ovaries, but also by estrogens synthesized locally from precursors such as cholesterol and testosterone through activity of the CYP450 enzyme aromatase. In the rat, tissues expressing aromatase that may be particularly critical for energy regulation include the liver, adipose tissue, hypothalamus, and even taste buds of the circumvallate papillae (Azcoitia et al., 2011, Belanger et al., 2002, Charlier et al., 2010, Cornil, 2009, Cornil and Charlier, 2010, Cornil et al., 2006, Sanghera et al., 1991, Shibuya et al., 2003, Simpson et al., 2000, Toyoshima et al., 2007). Thus, differences in responding in female rats could also reflect changes in local estrogen synthesis through aromatase activity that follow puberty.

The goal of the present studies was to examine whether ovarian or other estrogens contribute to the observed resistance to weight gain in adult female rats given high-intensity sweeteners. In Experiment 1, the ovaries of adult female rats were removed surgically and the effects of consuming dietary supplements sweetened with the high-intensity sweetener saccharin were compared to supplements sweetened with glucose in female rats. In Experiment 2, the goal was to examine whether blocking the activity of aromatase, thereby inhibiting local estrogen synthesis, affected responding to saccharin-versus glucose-sweetened yogurt diet supplements and whether the consequences of inhibiting local estrogen synthesis by surgical removal of the ovaries were different compared to after puberty. The aromatase inhibitor anastrozole was used because it is designed for oral delivery, and has been previously demonstrated to significantly suppress estrogen levels in OVX female rats at the dose employed (0.1 mg/kg; Dukes, 1997, Wang et al., 2009). In rats, anastrozole is typically delivered by gavage or provided in drinking water as the sole source of fluids, which can diminish the amount of fluid consumed and thereby impact food intake. Preliminary work demonstrated that when anastrozole or its vehicle (ethanol) was mixed into the dietary supplement we typically use, low-fat yogurt, animals reliably consumed the entire portion of yogurt, and therefore the entire dose of the drug. We took advantage of the willingness of animals to consume the drug voluntarily by dissolving it in the yogurt supplements, allowing us to avoid stress associated with repeated gavage or suppression of fluid intake associated with providing it in drinking water.

Section snippets

Experiment 1

Subjects were 25 adult female Sprague–Dawley rats approximately 70 days of age (Harlan, Indianapolis) that received ad libitum access to a standard laboratory chow (Lab Diets 5012) for one week after arrival in the lab. Then, all rats were anesthetized with ketamine and xylazine prior to bilateral ovariectomy (OVX), which was performed as described previously (Swithers et al., 2008). Butorphanol (2.5 mg/kg s.c.) was used for post-operative analgesia. Animals were allowed to recover for 3 weeks

Experiment 1

Weight gain in OVX female rats maintained on a standard laboratory chow diet (Fig. 1) was affected by the day of testing (Main effect of Day, F26, 598 = 273.7.4, p < .0000001), but not by the sweetener provided in the yogurt (Fs < 1). Total energy intake (yogurt plus chow) across the 28 days of yogurt exposure was also unaffected by the sweetener provided in the yogurt (F1, 23 = 1.36, p = 0.25; Means = 2254 ± 66 kcal for Glucose and 2148 ± 63 kcal for Saccharin groups).

Experiment 2

Body weight at the start of the experiment

Discussion

Previous work has demonstrated that when given access to a standard laboratory chow diet, adult female rats, unlike adult male rats, show minimal differences in body weight gain when provided with sweetened dietary supplements that do not reliably predict the delivery of calories (Swithers et al., submitted for publication). Results of the present study indicate that these differences in responding between males and females are not directly related to changes in ovarian hormones during

Acknowledgments

We thank Melissa McCurley, Natalie Rappaport, and Ethan Flint for technical assistance, Dr. Terry Davidson for his helpful comments and discussion and Dr. Robert Meisel for his comments on a previous version of the manuscript. Supported by NIH grants R01 DK076078 and P01 HD052112. The funding source had no role in study design; collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the article for publication.

References (96)

  • D.J. Costanzo et al.

    State-dependent retention produced with estrus in rats

    Physiol. Behav.

    (1995)
  • K.S. Curtis et al.

    Sex differences in behavioral taste responses to and ingestion of sucrose and NaCl solutions by rats

    Physiol. Behav.

    (2004)
  • K.S. Curtis et al.

    Estrogen increases the taste threshold for sucrose in rats

    Physiol. Behav.

    (2005)
  • C. Dalla et al.

    Sex differences in learning processes of classical and operant conditioning

    Physiol. Behav.

    (2009)
  • R. de Jongh et al.

    The effects of sex and neonatal maternal separation on fear-potentiated and light-enhanced startle

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2005)
  • K.J. Duffey et al.

    Dietary patterns matter: diet beverages and cardiometabolic risks in the longitudinal Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) Study

    Am. J. Clin. Nutr.

    (2012)
  • L.A. Eckel

    The ovarian hormone estradiol plays a crucial role in the control of food intake in females

    Physiol. Behav.

    (2011)
  • L. Fester et al.

    Estrogen-regulated synaptogenesis in the hippocampus: sexual dimorphism in vivo but not in vitro

    J. Steroid Biochem. Mol. Biol.

    (2012)
  • R.R. Gupta et al.

    Estrogen modulates sexually dimorphic contextual fear conditioning and hippocampal long-term potentiation (LTP) in rats(1)

    Brain Res.

    (2001)
  • R. Hiroi et al.

    Differential effects of ovarian steroids on anxiety versus fear as measured by open field test and fear-potentiated startle

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2006)
  • G.E. Hodes et al.

    Distinctive stress effects on learning during puberty

    Horm. Behav.

    (2005)
  • B.J. Hrupka et al.

    Ovariectomy and estradiol affect postingestive controls of sucrose licking

    Physiol. Behav.

    (1997)
  • N.J. Kenney et al.

    Effects of ovariectomy and subsequent estradiol replacement on intake of sweet solutions

    Physiol. Behav.

    (1980)
  • B. Leuner et al.

    High levels of estrogen enhance associative memory formation in ovariectomized females

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2004)
  • D.L. Maney et al.

    Estradiol-dependent modulation of auditory processing and selectivity in songbirds

    Front. Neuroendocrinol.

    (2011)
  • S. Maren et al.

    Sex differences in hippocampal long-term potentiation (LTP) and Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats: positive correlation between LTP and contextual learning

    Brain Res.

    (1994)
  • F. Moradpour et al.

    Anastrozole improved testosterone-induced impairment acquisition of spatial learning and memory in the hippocampal CA1 region in adult male rats

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2006)
  • J.A. Nettleton et al.

    Dietary patterns and incident cardiovascular disease in the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis

    Am. J. Clin. Nutr.

    (2009)
  • B.M. Nugent et al.

    Hormonally mediated epigenetic changes to steroid receptors in the developing brain: implications for sexual differentiation

    Horm. Behav.

    (2011)
  • J.G. Oberlander et al.

    Neural aromatization accelerates the acquisition of spatial memory via an influence on the songbird hippocampus

    Horm. Behav.

    (2004)
  • D.A. Pape-Zambito et al.

    Estrone and 17beta-estradiol concentrations in pasteurized-homogenized milk and commercial dairy products

    J. Dairy Sci.

    (2010)
  • J. Prange-Kiel et al.

    Direct and indirect effects of estrogen on rat hippocampus

    Neuroscience

    (2006)
  • C.R. Pryce et al.

    Effect of sex on fear conditioning is similar for context and discrete CS in Wistar, Lewis and Fischer rat strains

    Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav.

    (1999)
  • K. Shibuya et al.

    Hippocampal cytochrome P450s synthesize brain neurosteroids which are paracrine neuromodulators of synaptic signal transduction

    Biochim. Biophys. Acta

    (2003)
  • T.J. Shors et al.

    Estrogen-mediated effects on depression and memory formation in females

    J. Affect. Disord.

    (2003)
  • E. Simpson et al.

    The role of local estrogen biosynthesis in males and females

    Trends Endocrinol. Metab.

    (2000)
  • S.E. Swithers et al.

    Influence of ovarian hormones on development of ingestive responding to alterations in fatty acid oxidation in female rats

    Horm. Behav.

    (2008)
  • S.E. Swithers et al.

    High-intensity sweeteners and energy balance

    Physiol. Behav.

    (2010)
  • F. van Haaren et al.

    Behavioral differences between male and female rats: effects of gonadal hormones on learning and memory

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (1990)
  • G.N. Wade

    Gonadal hormones and behavioral regulation of body weight

    Physiol. Behav.

    (1972)
  • G.N. Wade et al.

    Gonadal effects on food intake and adiposity: a metabolic hypothesis

    Physiol. Behav.

    (1979)
  • J. Weinberg et al.

    Sex differences in biobehavioral responses to conflict in a taste aversion paradigm

    Physiol. Behav.

    (1982)
  • M.E. Wilson et al.

    Estrogen receptor-alpha gene expression in the cortex: sex differences during development and in adulthood

    Horm. Behav.

    (2011)
  • S.C. Woods et al.

    Pavlovian influences over food and drug intake

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2000)
  • D.L. Yuan et al.

    Estradiol accelerates extinction of a conditioned taste aversion in female and male rats

    Horm. Behav.

    (1999)
  • American Dietetic, A.

    Position of the American Dietetic Association: use of nutritive and nonnutritive sweeteners

    J. Am. Diet. Assoc.

    (2004)
  • L. Asarian et al.

    Modulation of appetite by gonadal steroid hormones

    Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci.

    (2006)
  • D.A. Bangasser et al.

    The hippocampus is necessary for enhancements and impairments of learning following stress

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (2007)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text