Linking place preferences with place meaning: An examination of the relationship between place motivation and place attachment

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.11.001Get rights and content

Abstract

Using data collected from residents surrounding a large urban park setting, we examined the relationship between their motivation to visit the park and their attachment to the setting. Based on the literature suggesting that natural environments provide humans with a variety of desired psychological, social, and physiological outcomes, we hypothesized that these outcomes would motivate respondents to interact with the park environment and facilitate the development of their attachment to the setting. Our data were analysed using covariance structure analysis. The results of this analysis offered partial support for our hypothesized model. Using multidimensional conceptualizations of motivation and place attachment, we observed that not all dimensions of motivation had a significant effect on the dimensions of place attachment. The valence of the significant predictors, however, was consistent with our hypotheses and prior literature.

Introduction

Place attachment has been used by scholars in a number of fields to describe the phenomena of human–place bonding. Their work has shown that the nature of humans’ attachment to place can be considered in a variety of ways. While authors have used a number of terms to describe the phenomena of human–place bonding (e.g. ‘sense of place’, ‘rootedness’, ‘insidedness’, ‘environmental embeddedness’), Low and Altman (1992) indicated that most conceptualizations, to varying degrees, include three components: affect, cognition, and practice. The affective component is most often reflected in emotional attachments to place, whereas the cognitive component concerns thoughts, knowledge, and beliefs related to place. Practice or conative action refers to the behaviors and activities that occur within spatial contexts.

In the context of natural resource management1 within the United States, both managers and researchers have recently begun to appreciate the construct's utility for addressing a variety of natural resource management issues. For example, in the context of stakeholder conflict, research has shown that competing claims to a resource or setting can be negotiated with an understanding of the bonds stakeholders share with the contested landscape (Bonaiuto, Carrus, Martorella, & Bonnes, 2002; Clark & Stein, 2003). An understanding of these bonds and the meanings that gird them, provides insight on why stakeholders value particular settings. With this understanding, managers are better able to facilitate compromises between affected groups. Thus, efforts to refine our understanding of place attachment and its formative processes have important implications for public land management.

With regard to understanding place attachment's antecedent processes, studies of residential settings (e.g. house, neighborhood, and city) have shown that the development of place attachment is influenced by a number of factors. For example, several authors have shown that place bonds develop over time in response to individual interaction with the environment (Hay, 1998; Milligan, 1998). Other work has reinforced the importance of the person–environment interaction and has illustrated that past experience and the memories of those experiences also contributes to the development of place bonding (Rowles, 1983; Vorkinn & Riese, 2001). Finally, studies have shown that social ties and memories of significant others can also contribute to the development of place bonds (Mesch & Manor, 1998; Hidalgo & Hernández, 2001; Uzzell, Pol, & Badenas, 2002). Research in natural settings, however, suggests that other processes may also be influential in fostering attachments to natural environments. For example, Kaplan and Kaplan (1989) have suggested that humans’ preference for nature is driven by the desire to satiate specific needs. They noted that natural environments offer a variety of psychological, social, and physiological benefits not typically found in developed or human-influenced environments. In addition to providing pleasure and satisfaction, they suggested that natural ‘settings support human functioning. They provide a context in which people can manage information effectively; they permit people to move about and explore with comfort and confidence. And, finally, such environments foster the recovery from mental fatigue. They permit tired individuals to regain effective functioning’ (p. 196). Given these benefits, it is possible that in natural settings humans’ attachment to the setting could emerge from their interaction with the setting—interactions driven by their desire to satiate specific needs. That is, humans are drawn to natural environments by the expectation of experiencing specific desired outcomes (i.e. psychological, social, and physiological). Through their engagements with the setting and others within the setting, psychological bonds between the self and the setting may then develop. To explore this issue further, we drew on motivation theory to explore the relationship between individuals’ motives for visiting a public recreation setting and their attachment to the setting.

Section snippets

Literature review

Research has shown that, for a variety or reasons, humans are attracted to natural environments (for reviews see Knopf (1983), Knopf (1987); Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989). These investigations have shown that when encountering or presented with images of natural environments, subjects experience a variety of positive psychological, social, and physiological outcomes. It is our contention that the expectation of these outcomes draws people toward specific natural environments and, over time,

Sample and study context

Data for our study were collected from subscribers to Cleveland Metroparks’ Emerald Necklace publication. Cleveland Metroparks is a public leisure service provider in suburban Cleveland, Ohio. The agency's mission consists of three elements: (a) environmental conservation (e.g. wildlife and habitat management), (b) outdoor education (e.g. environmental centers and programs), and (c) and recreation (e.g. nature trails, environmental appreciation, and open playing fields. Consistent with this

Model testing

The first step in our model testing procedure began with an examination of the measurement model (confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) in LISREL version 8.5) which examined the suitability of our hypothesized factor structure for these data.6 Both the fit indices (χ2

Discussion

The purpose of this investigation was to examine the relationship between individuals’ motivation to interact with natural settings and their attachment to these settings. Using data collected from subscribers to a monthly magazine published by a public leisure service provider, our findings illustrated that as respondents’ scores on the dimensions of motivation increased, so too did their attachment to the setting. Further, the strength of this relationship was not influenced by respondents’

References (88)

  • J.F. Sallis et al.

    Environmental and policy interventions to promote physical activity

    American Journal of Preventive Medicine

    (1998)
  • C.L. Twigger-Ross et al.

    Place and identity processes

    Journal of Environmental Psychology

    (1996)
  • R.S. Ulrich et al.

    Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments

    Journal of Environmental Psychology

    (1991)
  • R.C. Beck

    Motivationtheories and principles

    (2003)
  • P.M. Bentler

    Comparative fit indices in structural models

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1990)
  • R.D. Bixler et al.

    Environmental socializationQuantitative tests of the childhood play hypothesis

    Environment and Behavior

    (2002)
  • K.A. Bollen

    Structural equations with latent variables

    (1989)
  • A.M. Brandenburg et al.

    Your place, or mine? The effect of place creation on environmental values and landscape meanings

    Society and Natural Resource Management

    (1995)
  • K. Bricker et al.

    Level of specialization and place attachmentan exploratory study of whitewater recreationists

    Leisure Sciences

    (2000)
  • P.J. Brown et al.

    Wilderness recreation experiencethe Rawah case

    Journal of Leisure Research

    (1980)
  • W. Burch

    Two concepts for guiding recreation decisions

    Journal of Forestry

    (1964)
  • B.M. Byrne

    Structural equation modeling with LISREL, PRELIS, and SIMPLISbasic concepts, applications, and programming

    (1998)
  • B.M. Byrne et al.

    Testing for the equivalence of factor covariance and mean structuresthe issue of partial measurement invariance

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1989)
  • N. Cheek

    Toward a sociology of not-work

    Pacific Sociological Review

    (1971)
  • N. Cheek et al.

    The social organization of leisure in human society

    (1976)
  • J.K. Clark et al.

    Incorporating the natural landscape within an assessment of community attachment

    Forest Science

    (2003)
  • J.M. Cortina

    What is coefficient alpha? An examination of theory and applications

    Journal of Applied Psychology

    (1993)
  • T.C. Daniel

    Measuring the natural environmenta psychological approach

    American Psychologist

    (1990)
  • T.C. Daniel et al.

    Conditions for environmental perception researchreactions to Ward and Russell

    Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (1981)
  • D.A. Dillman

    Mail and Internet surveysthe tailored design method

    (2000)
  • B.L. Driver

    Quantification of outdoor recreationists’ preferences

  • Driver, B. L. (1983). Master list of items for recreation experience preference scales and domain. Unpublished...
  • Driver, B. L., & Brown, P. J. (1986). Probable personal benefits of outdoor recreation. In President's commission on...
  • B.L. Driver et al.

    Personality, outdoor recreation, and expected consequences

    Environment and Behavior

    (1977)
  • B.L. Driver et al.

    The paragraphs about leisure and recreation experience preference scalesresults from two inventories designed to access the breadth of perceived psychological benefits of leisure

  • B.L. Driver et al.

    Toward a behavioral interpretation of recreation, with implications for planning

  • B.W. Eisenhauer et al.

    Attachments to special places on public landsan analysis of activities, reason for attachments, and community connections

    Society and Natural Resources

    (2000)
  • A.R. Graefe et al.

    Notes on the stability of the factor structure of leisure meanings

    Leisure Sciences

    (1981)
  • T.W. Gruen et al.

    Relationship marketing activities, commitment, and membership behaviors in professional associations

    Journal of Marketing

    (2000)
  • P. Gustafson

    Meanings of placeeveryday experience and theoretical conceptualizations

    Journal of Environmental Psychology

    (2000)
  • R. Hay

    A rooted sense of place in cross-cultural perspective

    The Canadian Geographer

    (1998)
  • J.W. Hoelter

    The analysis of covariance structuresgoodness-of-fit indices

    Sociological Methods and Research

    (1983)
  • Holcomb, B. (1977). The perception of natural vs. built environments by young children. In Children, nature, and the...
  • N. Humpel et al.

    Environmental factors associated with adults’ participation in physical activitya review

    American Journal of Preventive Medicine

    (2002)
  • Cited by (406)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text