Elsevier

Cognition

Volume 129, Issue 1, October 2013, Pages 114-122
Cognition

Do social utility judgments influence attentional processing?

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.06.011Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We examine trustworthiness judgments of faces using an attentional-blink paradigm.

  • Strong appearance-based judgments enhance face memory but not pre-attentive processing.

  • Judgments based on behavior enhance both, but only if they are positive.

  • Thus, positive behavior but not appearance biases early attentional processing.

  • Early attentional biases shape information acquisition and ultimately social decisions.

Abstract

Research shows that social judgments influence decision-making in social environments. For example, judgments about an interaction partners’ trustworthiness affect a variety of social behaviors and decisions. One mechanism by which social judgments may influence social decisions is by biasing the automatic allocation of attention toward certain social partners, thereby shaping the information people acquire. Using an attentional blink paradigm, we investigate how trustworthiness judgments alter the allocation of attention to social stimuli in a set of two experiments. The first experiment investigates trustworthiness judgments based solely on a social partner’s facial appearance. The second experiment examines the effect of trustworthiness judgments based on experienced behavior. In the first, strong appearance-based judgments (positive and negative) enhanced stimulus recognizability but did not alter the size of the attentional blink, suggesting that appearance-based social judgments enhance face memory but do not affect pre-attentive processing. However, in the second experiment, in which judgments were based on behavioral experience rather than appearance, positive judgments enhanced pre-attentive processing of trustworthy faces. This suggests that a stimulus’s potential benefits, rather than its disadvantages, shape the automatic distribution of attentional resources. These results have implications for understanding how appearance- and behavior-based social cues shape attention distribution in social environments.

Introduction

Humans make hundreds of decisions every day. Often, these choices depend heavily on the signals people receive from their interaction partners (Behrens, Hunt, Woolrich, & Rushworth, 2008). For example, facial expressions contribute important information to appearance-based social decision-making. Research shows that faces displaying negative emotions, such as fear and anger, are pre-attentively processed (Eastwood et al., 2003, Fox et al., 2000, Öhman et al., 2001). This attentional negativity-bias is explained by an adaptive evolutionary drive to avoid threat (Cosmides & Tooby, 2000). However, recent research showing that valuable stimuli are also pre-attentively processed has begun to suggest that reward-related information may bias attention in a similar fashion (Anderson et al., 2011, Dux and Marois, 2009, Roelfsema et al., 2010) and that positive emotional expressions facilitate target detection (Hodsall, Viding, & Lavie, 2011). While threat detection may be important in many contexts, it may be less influential in the everyday social environments people typically experience.

One factor that may shape decision-making in ordinary social contexts is people’s judgments of those with whom they interact. These social judgments are important because they guide expectations about how a partner might behave (Cosmides, 1989, Cosmides and Tooby, 2000, Frith and Frith, 1999). Appearance-related social judgments are particularly influential (Willis & Todorov, 2006). For example, research shows that the degree to which an individual looks trustworthy influences a range of decisions including financial investments (van‘t Wout and Sanfey, 2008), the interpretation of verbal information (Hassin & Trope, 2000), wagering behavior (Schlicht, Shimojo, Camerer, Battaglia, & Nakayama, 2010), legal decisions (Porter, ten Brinke, & Gustaw, 2010), and voting (Olivola & Todorov, 2010).

Nonetheless, appearances are not always accurate (Porter, England, Juodis, van Brinke, & Wilson, 2008). Research has therefore begun to examine how behavioral experience alters social judgments. This work shows that people’s behavior significantly influences others’ judgments such that truthful, consistent, and prosocial behaviors lead to more positive interpersonal evaluations (Ames and Johar, 2009, Bayliss and Tipper, 2006, Heerey and Velani, 2010). Thus, these findings suggest that both appearance- and experience-based judgments influence social decisions.

Recently, research has begun to suggest that social judgments may be akin to economic value judgments (Chang, Doll, van‘t Wout, Frank & Sanfey, 2010) because they shape expectations about the utility or subjective desirability of interacting with a particular social partner. For example, the presence of social rewards such as genuine smiles increases stimulus utility and influences subsequent economic decisions (Shore & Heerey, 2011). Moreover, people assume that interaction partners who look attractive or trustworthy, or engage in prosocial behavior will provide positive outcomes and other social rewards (Delgado et al., 2005, Wilson and Eckel, 2006). This evidence therefore suggests that social cues lead to joint economic and social judgments of interaction partners, which subsequently influence decisions by biasing people’s expectations about those partners.

Social interactions, especially with multiple partners, contain more information than people can process (Foulsham, Cheng, Tracy, Henrich, & Kingstone, 2010). Therefore, biases based on social judgments may shape decision-making in more subtle ways as well. For example, research has shown that non-social reward cues change stimulus utility, and that this influences the pre-attentive processing of rewarded stimuli (Anderson et al., 2011, De Martino et al., 2009, Dux and Marois, 2009). If social utility judgments influence neural processing in a similar fashion, we predict that social stimuli eliciting strong judgments should likewise capture attention, suggesting one mechanism by which those stimuli shape social decisions and behavior. Specifically, the ability of an interaction partner to capture attention, even for a short while, may bias the information one gains during an interaction involving that partner. Therefore, social judgments may bias decision-making by guiding attention toward or away from particular partners, thereby determining the information people acquire and use in subsequent decisions.

Here, we ask how appearance – (Experiment 1) and behavior-based (Experiment 2) social utility judgments affect the allocation of attention to social stimuli. Understanding how such judgments shape the perception and attentional processing of stimuli provides an important clue about how social judgments influence decision-making processes. To measure differences between stimuli in terms of attention capture, we utilized an attentional blink (AB) paradigm (Raymond and O’Brien, 2009, Raymond et al., 1992). The AB is an elegant way of measuring the degree to which different stimuli automatically capture attention. In AB tasks, participants must detect two visual stimuli presented at varying time points in a rapid stream of images. If the second stimulus occurs within 500 ms of the first, it is often undetected (Chun & Potter, 1995), causing an impairment in perceptual encoding known as the attentional blink (Raymond et al., 1992). Interestingly, participants are less likely to miss a target presented within 500 ms of another when the target is emotionally salient (Keil & Ihssen, 2004). If social judgments increase the motivational or emotional salience of social stimuli this should cause those stimuli to capture attention, even when they are presented within the window of the attentional blink (i.e., within 500 ms following another target).

Section snippets

Experiment 1

Here, we investigate whether appearance-based social utility judgments affect the recognition of faces when they appear within versus after the window of the attentional blink. In this experiment, we use judgments of trustworthiness, as this trait is judged quickly, reliably and automatically from physical appearance (Berry and Brownlow, 1989, Engell et al., 2007, Olsen and Marshuetz, 2005, Todorov et al., 2009, Todorov et al., 2008, Willis and Todorov, 2006). We predict that when stimuli

Experiment 2

Although appearance-based social judgments influence behavior towards social partners, sound social decisions require a great deal more information than that conveyed by appearance alone. For example, it is unwise to make important decisions, such as choosing a babysitter, without any knowledge or experience of an individual’s behavior. Thus, when no other information is available, social decision-making may rely on appearance-based social judgments. However, as behavioral information becomes

General discussion

Together, these results show two important things. First, people who elicit strong social impressions, both positive and negative and regardless of whether these judgments are based on appearance or behavior, are more recognizable. Second, social utility judgments must be based on behavioral experience to alter pre-attentive processing. In Experiment 1, appearance-based social utility judgments, affected face recognition such that faces provoking stronger or more salient judgments (both

Acknowledgement

We would like to thank Philippa Beston for her help with data collection.

References (48)

  • D.R. Ames et al.

    I’ll know what you’re like when I see how you feel: How and when affective displays influence behavior-based impressions

    Psychological Science

    (2009)
  • B.A. Anderson et al.

    Value-driven attentional capture

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

    (2011)
  • J.A. Bargh et al.

    The automaticity of social life

    Current Direction in Psychological Science

    (2006)
  • A.P. Bayliss et al.

    Predictive gaze cues and personality judgments: Should eye trust you?

    Psychological Science

    (2006)
  • T.E.J. Behrens et al.

    Associative learning of social value

    Nature

    (2008)
  • D.S. Berry et al.

    Were the physiognomists right? Personality correlates of facial babyishness

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (1989)
  • C.S. Carver et al.

    Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1994)
  • M. Chun et al.

    A two-stage model for multiple target detection in serial visual presentation

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

    (1995)
  • M. Corbetta et al.

    Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain

    Nature Reviews Neuroscience

    (2002)
  • L. Cosmides et al.

    The cognitive neuroscience of social reasoning

  • B. De Martino et al.

    Enhanced processing of threat stimuli under limited attentional resources

    Cerebral Cortex

    (2009)
  • M.R. Delgado et al.

    Perceptions of moral character modulate the neural systems of reward during the trust game

    Nature Neuroscience

    (2005)
  • P.E. Dux et al.

    How humans search for targets through time: A review of data and theory from the attentional blink

    Attention, Perception and Psychophysics

    (2009)
  • J.D. Eastwood et al.

    Negative facial expression captures attention and disrupts performance

    Perception and Psychophysics

    (2003)
  • Cited by (5)

    • Visual consciousness of faces in the attentional blink: Knowledge-based effects of trustworthiness dominate over appearance-based impressions

      2020, Consciousness and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      In addition to visible emotional content – for instance, in the case of emotional facial expressions (Maratos, Mogg, & Bradley, 2008; De Martino, Kalisch, Rees, & Dolan, 2009) – the associated value may also lead to such prioritized processing. For example, higher detection rates were reported for faces associated with threat (Milders, Sahraie, Logan, & Donnellon, 2006) or reward (Raymond & O’Brien, 2009; Shore & Heerey, 2013). These findings suggest that social knowledge about a person as a form of associated affective value, and facial trustworthiness as a form of (more strongly) visually based affective value, may also influence detection rates in the AB.

    • The influence of prior reputation and reciprocity on dynamic trust-building in adults with and without autism spectrum disorder

      2018, Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      Unbeknownst to the participants, virtual counterparts differed in their level of reciprocity: two counterparts were programmed to play a cooperative strategy, returning higher sums than the participants initially invested (r × i × 4 > i), and the remaining two were destined to play, in every round, an individualistic strategy, never returning more EMUs than the participants invested (r × i × 4 ≤ i). To improve ecological validity (Lis et al., 2016; Shore & Heerey, 2013), counterparts varied their return ratio r randomly across rounds within predefined margins. Cooperative counterparts returned between 37.5% and 50% of the quadrupled investment i × 4 (46.4% ± 0.03), and individualistic counterparts returned between 12.5% and 25% (17.92% ± 0.04) of i × 4.

    View full text