Elsevier

Child Abuse & Neglect

Volume 77, March 2018, Pages 222-231
Child Abuse & Neglect

Are emotions controllable? Maltreated and non-maltreated youth’s implicit beliefs about emotion and aggressive tendencies

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.01.010Get rights and content

Abstract

Although child maltreatment places youth at substantial risk for difficulties with emotion regulation and aggression, not all maltreated youth show these adverse effects, raising important questions about characteristics that discriminate those who do versus do not evidence long-term negative outcomes. The present investigation examined whether implicit beliefs about emotion moderated the association between maltreatment and aggression. Maltreated (n = 59) and community-matched (n = 66) youth were asked regarding their beliefs about emotion and aggressive behaviors. Beliefs about emotion were more strongly associated with aggression among maltreated youth, particularly physically abused youth. Maltreated youth who believed they had poor ability to control emotion reported significantly higher levels of aggression than comparison youth. However, maltreated youth who believed they had high ability to control emotion did not differ significantly in aggression from that of comparison youth. Findings offer unique insight into a factor that may increase or buffer maltreated youth’s risk for aggression and thus highlight potential directions for interventions to reduce aggressive tendencies.

Introduction

Child maltreatment constitutes one of the most serious violations of children’s needs and well-being, the consequences of which extend across virtually all domains of functioning (Cicchetti, 2013; Cicchetti & Lynch, 1995). One such consequence, which emerges in early childhood and persists well into adulthood, involves ineffective regulation of emotions (Burns, Jackson, & Harding, 2010; Cummings, Hennessy, & Rabideau, 1994; Kim-Spoon, Cicchetti, & Rogosch, 2013; Stevens et al., 2013). Maltreated youth exhibit greater negative emotionality, particularly anger reactivity, than non-maltreated youth (Gunnar & Donzella, 2002), and often have difficulties modulating their responses to emotional situations (Kim-Spoon, Haskett, Longo, & Nice, 2012, Maughan & Cicchetti, 2002). These regulatory difficulties, in turn, contribute to a host of problematic behaviors, including aggression, delinquency, and other externalizing tendencies (Silk, Steinberg, & Morris, 2003; Eisenberg et al., 2001; Jaffee, 2017; Mullin & Hinshaw, 2007).

Yet, not all maltreated youth manifest emotional dysregulation. Some demonstrate resilience, functioning competently in social and emotional domains, despite having endured trauma and adversity (Flores, Cicchetti, & Rogosch, 2005; Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1997; Cicchetti, 2010; Haskett, Nears, Sabourin Ward, & McPherson, 2006). Accordingly, it is of considerable interest to identify factors that influence emotion regulation in maltreated youth, leading some to exhibit minimal deficits and others to fall into a pattern of maladaptation. In the present investigation, we focused on one such unexplored factor: implicit beliefs about emotion. We assessed whether such beliefs differed between maltreated and comparison youth and whether they predicted youth’s aggressive tendencies.

Broadly, implicit beliefs about emotion refer to individuals’ perceptions of the controllability of their emotions, with such beliefs falling along a continuum (Tamir, John, Srivastava, & Gross, 2007; Romero, Master, Paunesku, Dweck, & Gross, 2014). At one end are individuals who endorse an entity theory of emotion, believing that their emotions are beyond their control; at the other are individuals who endorse an incremental theory, believing that their emotions can be changed through effort (e.g. Tamir et al., 2007; De Castella et al., 2013).

Whether individuals tend toward entity or incremental beliefs has significant implications for their emotion regulation tendencies and behavioral functioning. In young adults, for example, those who view emotions as impossible to modify (i.e., who hold an entity belief) tend to appraise negative affect as threatening (Kappes & Schikowski, 2013) and often fail to regulate their emotions or behaviors in anticipation of emotionally-arousing situations (Tamir et al., 2007). These individuals, moreover, are prone to managing their affect by avoiding rather than actively confronting unpleasant emotional stimuli, a tendency that is ultimately associated with greater psychological distress (Shallcross, Troy, Boland, & Mauss, 2010; Shahar & Herr, 2011). Conversely, young adults who view emotions as under their control (i.e., who hold an incremental belief) are more likely to attempt to alter their emotional responses both in anticipation of an emotional event and actively as the event unfolds. Such individuals, as well, are more effective and confident while doing so (De Castella et al., 2013; Deng, Sang, & Chen, 2015; Kappes & Schikowski, 2013; Schroder, Dawood, Yalch, Donnellan, & Moser, 2015; Kneeland, Dovidio, Joormann, & Clark, 2016; Tamir et al., 2007), and they generally utilize regulation strategies that are adaptive in many settings, such as cognitive reappraisal (i.e., reframing how one thinks about an emotional event to change its emotional impact) (Schroder et al., 2015). Finally, adults who hold incremental beliefs report fewer mental health symptoms, higher self-esteem, and better social adjustment than adults who hold entity beliefs (De Castella et al, 2013; Schroder et al., 2015).

Most work on implicit beliefs of emotion has focused on adults. The few studies to date examining youth, however, have yielded similar findings. Specifically, not only do youth’s beliefs about emotion appear to fall along the same entity to incremental continuum (Romero et al., 2014), but holding entity beliefs about emotion is also associated with poorer mental health in youth, including greater depressive symptoms and less flexible and adaptive emotion regulation. Incremental beliefs, on the other hand, are associated with youth’s greater emotional well-being and use of regulation strategies that are well-suited to the situation (Romero et al., 2014; Schleider & Weisz, 2016; Suveg & Zeman, 2004).

Although extant work with adults and youth has largely concerned the links between implicit beliefs about emotion and both general regulation tendencies and internalizing problems (e.g., depression), there are several reasons to suspect that implicit beliefs should also predict externalizing problems, such as aggression. First, entity theories in other domains (i.e., believing that personality, peer relations, or intelligence are outside one’s control) are generally predictive of increased externalizing tendencies among a wide range of youth (see Schleider, Abel, & Weisz, 2015 for a review). For example, youth who view others’ personalities or their peer relationships as unchangeable tend to evaluate others’ wrong-doing more harshly and endorse more aggressive retaliation against others when provoked (Gervey, Chiu, Hong, & Dweck, 1999; Erdley & Dweck, 1993; Levy & Dweck, 1999). Indeed, interventions designed to shift youths’ implicit beliefs about personality from entity towards incremental perceptions (i.e., from believing people’s personality traits are fixed to believing that they are malleable) have shown success in reducing youths’ subsequent reactive aggression, that is, impulsive and emotionally-charged aggressive responses to threat (Yeager, Trzesniewski, & Dweck, 2013).

Second, entity beliefs may foster hostile attribution biases (i.e., a tendency to readily perceive hostility in ambiguous situations) (Yeager, Miu, Powers, & Dweck, 2013), which in turn are predictive of aggression, especially reactive forms (Crick & Dodge, 1996). Studies of implicit beliefs about personality, for example, suggest that youth with entity theories are more likely to interpret even ambiguous provocation from peers as hostile and respond to this perceived provocation with aggression (Yeager, Trzesniewski, Tirri, Nokelainen, & Dweck 2011; Yeager et al., 2013). When these youth are taught to adopt more incremental beliefs about others through interventions, they often show subsequent reductions in both hostile attributions and reactive aggression (Yeager et al., 2013). And third, as mentioned, entity beliefs specifically about emotion are related to less adaptive and efficient emotion regulation, and poor regulation is a significant risk factor for aggression (Eisenberg, Spinrad, & Eggum, 2010).

As a final note, the links between implicit beliefs and aggression may be most relevant when considering children’s engagement in reactive, rather than proactive, aggression. Reactive aggression is impulsive and motivated by anger, retaliation, or defensiveness to perceived or actual provocation (Hubbard, McAuliffe, Morrow, & Romano, 2010; Cima & Raine, 2009). Beliefs about the controllability of emotion may play a specific and important role in children’s ability to inhibit these emotionally-driven responses. Children’s proactive aggression, on the other hand, should be less if at all related to their beliefs about emotion, given that proactive aggression typically involves unemotional and deliberate actions to achieve a goal (Hubbard et al., 2010). These possibilities need to be tested directly.

Some evidence points to the possibility that maltreatment might uniquely predispose children to entity beliefs, and that these beliefs may have implications for children’s aggressive behavior. Considerable research indicates, for example, that maltreated children are likely to grow up in environments in which poor emotion regulation skills are modeled by caregivers (Calkins & Fox, 2002). This may be especially true of physically abused youth, whose parents tend to experience and display high levels of anger, frustration, and hostility (McPherson, Lewis, Lynn, Haskett, & Behrend, 2009; Wilson, Rack, Shi, & Norris, 2008), and who are uniquely prone to aggressive outbursts (Shields & Cicchetti, 1998). Maltreating parents are also less likely to discuss emotions with their children than non-maltreating parents and less likely to validate and support their children’s experiences of emotions (Shipman & Zeman, 2001; Shipman et al., 2007). In combination, this may limit children’s opportunities to develop positive emotion regulation skills and understanding of others’—and their own— emotions. Indeed, when faced with challenging situations that warrant emotion regulation, maltreated youth often lack the skills necessary to effectively understand and control their emotions (Shields & Cicchetti, 1998; Kim & Cicchetti, 2010), and as a result, are at heightened risk for responding toward others with aggression (Dodge, Bates, & Pettit, 1990). It would thus be unsurprising if maltreated children developed views that their emotions and ensuing behaviors are beyond their control. These views, in turn, likely diminish maltreated children’s attempts at regulating their aggression.

At the same time, there are a number of other differences between maltreated and non-maltreated children that may further influence both their beliefs about emotion and aggressive tendencies. One, for example, concerns broader beliefs about control, beyond simply control over emotions. Maltreated youth are disproportionately likely to believe that current and future outcomes are beyond their control (Barahal et al., 1981; Roazzi, Attili, Di Pentima, & Toni, 2016). Such beliefs are associated with decreases in adaptive emotion regulation and coping (Skinner and Zimmer-Gembeck, 2010; Cortez & Bugental, 1994) and increased feelings of frustration and hostility (Patrick, Skinner, & Connell, 1993; Luthar, 1991). These again may contribute to aggressive responses, particularly when children feel threatened (McCauley, Mitchell, Burke, & Moss, 1988; Muris, Schouten, Meesters, & Gijsbers, 2003; Moyal, 1977). To understand how maltreated children’s implict beliefs specifically about emotions relate to their aggressive tendencies, it is important to take into account their general perceptions of control as well.

A second characteristic that often diverges between maltreated and non-maltreated children and that may influence aggression concerns hyperactivity. Regardless of maltreatment status, hyperactivity is a powerful predictor of a wide range of anger- and aggressive-related behaviors, including poor emotion regulation, low frustration tolerance, and increased impulsivity (Hill, Degnan, Calkins, & Keane, 2006; Maedgen & Carlson, 2000). Moreover, maltreated youth are prone to symptoms of hyperactivity and inattention (Famularo, Kinscherff, & Fenton, 1992; Ouyang, Fang, Mercy, Perou, & Grosse, 2008; Briscoe-Smith & Hinshaw, 2006), and hyperactvity may be a manifestation of other mental health problems (e.g., post-traumatic stress symptoms) that result from maltreatment. Given its potential influence and significance, therefore, in maltreated youth’s general functioning, it is important to control for hyperactivity when examining aggressive tendencies in this population.

Section snippets

Present study

We interviewed maltreated youth and a matched community sample of comparison youth, ages 10–17, about their background, implicit theories of emotion, behavioral adjustment, and general perceptions of control. Our main prediction was that maltreatment would interact with youth entity beliefs to predict aggressive tendencies. Specifically, we expected maltreated youth to hold greater entity beliefs than comparison youth, and that, for the maltreated youth, entity beliefs would be more strongly

Method

The present data were collected as a part of a large project concerning emotion regulation in maltreated children aged 6–17 years. However, only youth age 10 and older were included in the present report. For one, the Implicit Beliefs about Emotion Scale (De Castella et al, 2013; Tamir et al., 2007) has only been used with adults and adolescents, and has not been validated with or administered to younger children, likely due to the measure’s statements being too complex for younger ages.

Preliminary analyses

The maltreated and comparison samples were comparable in age, t (123) = -1.79, p = 0.08, gender, χ2(1) = 0.19, p = 0.67, and percentage Latino ethnicity, χ2(1) = 1.11, p = 0.29. Gender was unrelated to implicit beliefs about emotion and self-reported aggressive behaviors and is not considered further. Given the potential for youths’ tendencies toward impulsive or inattentive behavior to influence a range of their perceptions and actions, we next evaluated whether symptoms of hyperactivity

Discussion

The overarching purpose of the present study was to examine whether youth’s beliefs about the controllability of their emotions were predictive of their self-reported aggression and whether these associations varied between maltreated and comparison youth. Some results confirmed expectations, while others did not. For one, although maltreated and comparison youth did not generally differ in their beliefs about the controllability of their emotions or their broader perceptions of control over

References (80)

  • J.L. Schleider et al.

    Implicit theories and youth mental health problems: A random-effects meta-analysis

    Clinical Psychology Review

    (2015)
  • B. Shahar et al.

    Depressive symptoms predict inflexibly high levels of experiential avoidance in response to daily negative affect: A daily diary study

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2011)
  • A.J. Shallcross et al.

    Let it be: Accepting negative emotional experiences predicts decreased negative affect and depressive symptoms

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2010)
  • S.R. Wilson et al.

    Comparing physically abusive, neglectful, and non-maltreating parents during interactions with their children: A meta-analysis of observational studies

    Child Abuse & Neglect

    (2008)
  • L.S. Aiken et al.

    Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions

    (1991)
  • R.M. Barahal et al.

    The social cognitive development of abused children

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (1981)
  • D. Barnett et al.

    Defining child maltreatment: The interface between policy and research

  • L.S. Blackwell et al.

    Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention

    Child Development

    (2007)
  • E.E. Burns et al.

    Child Maltreatment, emotion regulation, and posttraumatic stress: The impact of emotional abuse

    Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma

    (2010)
  • S.D. Calkins et al.

    Self-regulatory processes in early personality development: A multilevel approach to the study of childhood social withdrawal and aggression

    Development and Psychopathology

    (2002)
  • D. Cicchetti

    Resilience under conditions of extreme stress: A multilevel perspective

    World Psychiatry

    (2010)
  • D. Cicchetti

    Annual research review: Resilient functioning in maltreated children--past, present, and future perspectives

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines

    (2013)
  • D. Cicchetti et al.

    Emotions and emotion regulation in developmental psychopathology

    Development and Psychopathology

    (1995)
  • D. Cicchetti et al.

    Failures in the expectable environment and their impact on individual development: The case of child maltreatment: risk, disorder, and adaptation

  • D. Cicchetti et al.

    The role of self-organization in the promotion of resilience in maltreated children

    Development and Psychopathology

    (1997)
  • V.L. Cortez et al.

    Children's visual avoidance of threat: A strategy associated with low social control

    Merrill-Palmer Quarterly

    (1994)
  • N.R. Crick et al.

    Social information-processing mechanisms on reactive and proactive aggression

    Child Development

    (1996)
  • E.M. Cummings et al.

    Responses of physically abused boys to interadult anger involving their mothers

    Development and Psychopathology

    (1994)
  • X. Deng et al.

    Implicit beliefs about emotion regulation and their relations with emotional experiences among Chinese adolescents

    International Journal of Behavioral Development

    (2015)
  • K.A. Dodge

    Translational science in action: Hostile attributional style and the development of aggressive behavior problems

    Development and Psychopathology

    (2006)
  • K.A. Dodge et al.

    Mechanisms in the cycle of violence

    Science

    (1990)
  • N. Eisenberg et al.

    The relations of regulation and emotionality to children’s externalizing and internalizing problem behavior

    Child Development

    (2001)
  • N. Eisenberg et al.

    Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to children's maladjustment

    Annual Review of Clinical Psychology

    (2010)
  • C.A. Erdley et al.

    Children's implicit personality theories as predictors of their social judgments

    Child Development

    (1993)
  • E. Flores et al.

    Predictors of resilience in maltreated and nonmaltreated latino children

    Developmental Psychology

    (2005)
  • B.M. Gervey et al.

    Differential use of person information in decisions about guilt versus innocence: The role of implicit theories

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (1999)
  • R. Goodman

    The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry

    (1997)
  • A.F. Hayes

    Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-Based Approach

    (2013)
  • A.L. Hill et al.

    Profiles of externalizing behavior problems for boys and girls across preschool: The roles of emotion regulation and inattention

    Developmental Psychology

    (2006)
  • J.A. Hubbard et al.

    Reactive and proactive aggression in childhood and adolescence: Precursors, outcomes, processes, experiences, and measurement

    Journal of Personality

    (2010)
  • Cited by (9)

    • Initial impact of the California continuum of care reform act on youth's experiences in out-of-home care

      2022, Children and Youth Services Review
      Citation Excerpt :

      The present study uses administrative data obtained as part of a larger project on socio-emotional development in high-risk youth. Maltreated youth living in county-wide temporary residential facilities following removal from caregiver custody due to substantiated cases of maltreatment were recruited to take part in the project (Dickerson et al., 2018). Between 2016 and 2018, 439 youth completed in-person interviews about a range of experiences and functioning.

    • Do children unintentionally report maltreatment? Comparison of disclosures of neglect versus sexual abuse

      2022, Child Abuse and Neglect
      Citation Excerpt :

      t-Test comparisons indicated that the two samples were similar in age t(131.40) = 0.63, p = .53, but there were more females than males in the sexual abuse sample, t(115.44) = −4.40, p < .001. Following approval from the Presiding Judge of Juvenile court to access and anonymize children's information, and university ethical approval to utilize the archived anonymized materials, case details were coded for children living in out of home care and had participated in other ongoing studies of maltreatment and legal involvement effects on children (e.g., Dickerson et al., 2018; Malloy et al., 2016; Quas et al., 2009; Wandrey et al., 2012). From these, case files were reviewed to identify children within the target age range who met the study criteria.

    • Brief sel interventions at school: Integrating research into practice

      2021, Brief SEL Interventions at School: Integrating Research into Practice
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text