Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The Interactive Effect of Parental Education on Language Production

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examines the interactive effect of mother’s and father’s education on childhood language development. Parents of sixteen- and twenty-month-old children (N = 48) completed measures on their children’s language production (MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory: Words and Sentences) as well as basic demographic information. There were variations in language production according to maternal education for only the older children. There was also an interaction between maternal and paternal education; children of parents with heterogeneous levels of education (that is, only one parent with a 4-year degree) had higher levels of language production than parents with homogeneous levels of education (that is, either both parents with a degree or both parents without a degree). Surprisingly, children with homogeneous levels of parental education were the ones who scored the lowest on measures of language production. This may be due to less effective parenting at both the low and high parental education levels or because disparity in parental education positively affects the home learning environment.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Arriaga, R. I., Fenson, L., Cronan, T., & Pethick, S. J. (1998). Scores on the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory of children from low- and middle-income families. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 209–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baum, S., & Payea, K. (2005). Education pays 2004: The benefits of higher education for individuals and society (Rev ed.). Washington, DC: College Board.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bee, H. L., Barnard, K. E., Eyres, S. J., Gray, C. A., Hammond, M. A., Spietz, A. L., et al. (1982). Prediction of IQ and language skill from perinatal status, child performance, family characteristics, and mother-infant interaction. Child Development, 53, 1134–1156.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meaning of words. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M. H., & Haynes, O. M. (1998). Vocabulary competence in early childhood: measurement, latent construct, and predictive validity. Child Development, 69, 654–671.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, O. M., & Painter, K. M. (1998). Sources of child vocabulary competence: a multivariate model. Journal of Child Language, 25, 367–393.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 371–399.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks-Gunn, J., Han, W., & Waldfogel, J. (2002). Maternal employment and child cognitive outcomes in the first three years of life: The NICHD study of early child care. Child Development, 73, 1052–1072.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burchinal, M. R., & Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (2007). Maternal employment and child cognitive outcomes: the importance of analytic approach. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1140–1155.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, T. F., Dollaghan, C. A., Rockette, H. E., Paradise, J. L., Feldman, H. M., Shriberg, L. D., et al. (2003). Risk factors for speech delay of unknown origin in 3-year-old children. Child Development, 74, 346–357.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, A., Bornstein, M. H., & Leach, D. B. (2005). Assessing early communicative ability: a cross-reporter cumulative score for the MacArthur CDI. Journal of Child Language, 32, 735–758.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Delgado, C. E. F., Vagi, S. J., & Scott, K. G. (2005). Early risk factors for speech and language impairments. Exceptionality, 13, 173–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dollaghan, C. A., Campbell, T. F., Paradise, J. L., Feldman, H. M., Janosky, J. E., Pitcairn, D. N., et al. (1999). Maternal education and measures of early speech and language. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 1432–1443.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ensminger, M. E., & Fothergill, K. (2003). A decade of measuring SES: What it tells us and where to go from here. In M. H. Bornstein & R. H. Bradley (Eds.), Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development (pp. 13–27). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farkas, G., & Beron, K. (2004). The detailed age trajectory of oral vocabulary knowledge: differences by class and race. Social Science Research, 33, 464–497.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, S., Bates, E., Thal, D., & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59.

  • Goldberg, W. A., Prause, J., Lucas-Thompson, R., & Himsel, A. (2008). Maternal employment and children’s achievement in context: a meta-analysis of four decades of research. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 77–108.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldfield, B. A., & Reznick, J. S. (1990). Early lexical acquisition: rate, content, and the vocabulary spurt. Journal of Child Language, 17, 171–183.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gottfried, A. W., Gottfried, A. E., Bathurst, K., Guerin, D. W., & Parramore, M. M. (2003). Socioeconomic status in children’s development and family environment: Infancy through adolescence. In M. H. Bornstein & R. H. Bradley (Eds.), Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development (pp. 189–207). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1992). American parenting of language-learning children: persisting differences in family-child interactions observed in natural home environments. Developmental Psychology, 28, 1096–1105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experiences of young American children. Baltimore: Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74, 1368–1378.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26, 55–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoff, E., & Tian, C. (2005). Socioeconomic status and cultural influences on language. Journal of Communication Disorders, 38, 271–278.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1998). The relation of birth order and socioeconomic status to children’s language experience and language development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 603–629.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hooper, S. R., Burchinal, M. R., Roberts, J. E., Zeisel, S., & Neebe, E. C. (1998). Social and family risk factors for infant development at one year: an application of the cumulative risk model. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 19, 85–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kloth, S., Janssen, P., Kraaimaat, F., & Brutten, G. J. (1998). Communicative styles of mothers interacting with their preschool-age children: a factor analytic study. Journal of Child Language, 25, 149–168.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luthar, S. S., & Becker, B. E. (2002). Privileged but pressured? A study of affluent youth. Child Development, 73, 1593–1610.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luthar, S. S., & Latendresse, S. J. (2005). Children of the affluent: challenges to well-being. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 49–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McMurray, B. (2007). Defusing the childhood vocabulary explosion. Science, 317, 631.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miser, T. M. & Hupp, J. M. (2011). The influence of SES, home environment, and childcare on child language abilities. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2000). The relation of child care to cognitive and language development. Child Development, 71, 960–980.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pan, B. A., Rowe, M. L., Singer, J. D., & Snow, C. E. (2005). Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development, 76, 763–782.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pancsofar, N., & Vernon-Feagans, L. (2006). Mother and father language input to young children: contributions to later language development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27, 571–587.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perna, L. W. (2005). The benefits of higher education: sex, racial/ethnic, and socioeconomic group differences. The Review of Higher Education, 29, 23–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prathanee, B., Thinkhamrop, B., & Dechongkit, S. (2007). Factors associated with specific language impairment and later language development during early life: a literature review. Clinical Pediatrics, 46, 22–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, E., Bornstein, M. H., Slater, A. M., & Barrett, J. (1999). Early cognitive development and parental education. Infant and Child Development, 8, 49–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, M. L., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009). Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in child vocabulary size at school entry. Science, 323, 951–953.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L., Jorm, A. F., Maclean, R., Mathews, R., & Waterman, B. (1983). Early reading achievement, oral language ability, and a child’s home background. Australian Psychologist, 18, 75–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • St. Sauver, J. L., Barbaresi, W. J., Katusic, S. K., Colligan, R. C., Weaver, A. L., & Jacobsen, S. J. (2004). Early life risk factors for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a population-based cohort study. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 79, 1124–1131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stanton-Chapman, T. L., Chapman, D. A., Bainbridge, N. L., & Scott, K. G. (2002). Identification of early risk factors for language impairment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 23, 390–405.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomblin, J., Records, N., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E., & O’Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 40, 1245–1260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vasilyeva, M., Waterfall, H., & Huttenlocher, J. (2008). Emergence of syntax: commonalities and differences across children. Developmental Science, 11, 84–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Klohnen, E. C., Casillas, A., Simms, E. N., Haig, J., & Berry, D. A. (2004). Match makers and deal breakers: analyses of assortative mating in newlywed couples. Journal of Personality, 72, 1029–1068.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, K. R. (1982). The relation between socioeconomic status and academic achievement. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 461–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgement

We thank Robert Haaf for his contributions to the original project and Melissa Jungers for her helpful comments on an earlier draft of the manuscript. This data was collected as part of a larger study conducted by the first author (Hupp, Infant Behavior & Development, 2008, Vol 31, 511-517). A portion of the data was presented at the 2009 Conference of the Society for Research in Child Development in Denver.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Julie M. Hupp.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hupp, J.M., Munala, L., Kaffenberger, J.A. et al. The Interactive Effect of Parental Education on Language Production. Curr Psychol 30, 312–323 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-011-9118-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-011-9118-x

Keywords

Navigation