Skip to main content
Log in

Modeling self-regulated learning as learners doing learning science: How trace data and learning analytics help develop skills for self-regulated learning

  • Published:
Metacognition and Learning Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Metacognition is the engine of self-regulated learning. At the object level, learners seek information and choose learning tactics and strategies they forecast will develop knowledge. At the meta level, learners gather and analyze data about learning events to draw conclusions, such as: Is this tactic a good fit to conditions? Was it effective? Was effort required reasonable? Is my ability publicly exposed? As data accumulate, learners shape, re-shape and refine a personal theory about optimal learning. Thus, self-regulating learners are learning scientists. However, without training and tools on which “professional” learning scientists rely, learners’ N = me research programs are naïve and scruffy. Merging models of tasks, cognition, metacognition and motivation, I describe software tools, approaches to analyzing data and learning analytics designed to serve three goals: supporting self-regulating learners’ metacognition in N = me research, accelerating professional learning scientists’ research, and boosting synergy among learners and learning scientists to accelerate progress in learning science.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. W. H. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, J. L., & Karabenick, S. A. (2016). Construct validity of self-reported metacognitive learning strategies. Educational Assessment, 21(1), 19–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Harwell, K. W. (2019). Deliberate practice and proposed limits on the effects of practice on the acquisition of expert performance: Why the original definition matters and recommendations for future research. Frontiers in Psychology, 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02396

  • Fiorella, L. (2020). The science of habit and its implications for student learning and well-being. Educational Psychology Review, 32, 1–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, A. J., Medaglia, J. D., & Jeronimus, B. F. (2018). Lack of group-to-individual generalizability is a threat to human subjects research. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 115, E6106–E6115. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1711978115

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. (1976). Metacognitive aspects of problem solving. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.), The nature of intelligence (pp. 231–236). Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H., Friedrichs, A. G., & Hoyt, J. D. (1970). Developmental changes in memorization processes. Cognitive Psychology, 1(4), 324–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gitelman, L. (Ed.). (2013). “Raw data” is an oxymoron. The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A. N., & Kuhl, P. K. (1999). The scientist in the crib: Minds, brains, and how children learn. William Morrow & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, J. T. (1965). Memory and the feeling-of-knowing experience. Journal of Educational Psychology, 56(4), 208–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, J. T. (1967). Memory and the memory-monitoring process. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 685–691.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hartwig, M. K., & Dunlosky, J. (2017). Category learning judgments in the classroom: Can students judge how well they know course topics? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 49, 80–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karabenick, S. A., Wooley, M. E., Friedel, J. M., Ammon, B. V., Blazevski, J., Rhee Bonney, C., De Groot, E. V., Gilbert, M. C., Musu, L., Kempler, T. M., & Kelly, K. L. (2007). Cognitive processing of self-report items in educational research: Do they think what we mean? Educational Psychologist, 42(3), 139–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, C., Siemens, G., Wise, A., & Gašević, D. (Eds.). (2021). Handbook of learning analytics (2nd ed.). Society for Learning Analytics Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, S., Zheng, J., Lajoie, S. P., & Wiseman, J. (2021). Examining the relationship between emotion variability, self-regulated learning, and task performance in an intelligent tutoring system. Educational Technology Research and Development, 69(2), 673–692.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miele, D. B., & Scholer, A. A. (2018). The role of metamotivational monitoring in motivation regulation. Educational Psychologist, 53(1), 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, 26, 125–141.

  • Piaget, J. (1950). The psychology of intelligence. Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saint, J., Fan, Yl., Singh, S., Gašević, D., & Pardo, A. (2021). Using process mining to analyse self-regulated learning: A systematic analysis of four algorithms. Learning Analytics and Knowledge LAK21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salomon, G., & Perkins, D. N. (1989). Rocky roads to transfer: Rethinking mechanisms of a neglected phenomenon. Educational Psychologist, 24, 113–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shannon, C. (1953). The lattice theory of information. Transactions of the IRE Professional Group on Information Theory, 1(1), 105–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Statistics Canada. (2021). Postsecondary enrolments, by international standard classification of education, institution type, classification of instructional programs, STEM and BHASE groupings, status of student in Canada, age group and gender. Retrieved from https://www150.statcan.gc.ca/t1/tbl1/en/tv.action?pid=3710016301. Accessed 5 May 2022.

  • Weiner, B. (2010). The development of an attribution-based theory of motivation: A history of ideas. Educational Psychologist, 45, 28–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winne, P. H. (1987). Why process-product research cannot explain process-product findings and a proposed remedy: The cognitive mediational paradigm. Teaching and Teacher Education, 3, 333–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winne, P. H. (1995a). Inherent details in self-regulated learning. Educational Psychologist, 30, 173–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winne, P. H. (1995b). Self regulation is ubiquitous but its forms vary with knowledge. Educational Psychologist, 30, 223–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winne, P. H. (1997). Experimenting to bootstrap self-regulated learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 397–410.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winne, P. H. (2010). Bootstrapping learner’s self-regulated learning. Psychological Test and Assessment Modeling, 52, 472–490.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winne, P. H. (2017). Leveraging big data to help each learner upgrade learning and accelerate learning science. Teachers College Record, 119(3), 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winne, P. H. (2018). Cognition and metacognition within self-regulated learning. In D. Schunk & J. Greene (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation of learning and performance (2nd ed., pp. 36–48). Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winne, P. H. (2019). Paradigmatic dimensions of instrumentation and analytic methods in research on self-regulated learning. Computers in Human Behavior, 96, 285–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winne, P. H. (2020a). A proposed remedy for grievances about self-report methodologies. Frontline Learning Research, 8, 165–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winne, P. H. (2020b). Construct and consequential validity for learning analytics based on trace data. Computers in Human Behavior, 112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2020.106457

  • Winne, P. H., & Azevedo, R. (2022). Metacognition and self-regulated learning. In R. K. Sawyer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of the learning sciences (3rd ed., pp. 93–113). Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Winne, P. H., & Marzouk, Z. (2019). Learning strategies and self-regulated learning. In J. Dunlosky & K. Rawson (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of cognition and education (pp. 696–715). Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Winne, P. H., Gupta, L., & Nesbit, J. C. (1994). Exploring individual differences in studying strategies using graph theoretic statistics. Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 40, 177–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winne, P. H., Teng, K., Chang, D., Lin, M.P.-C., Marzouk, Z., Nesbit, J. C., Patzak, A., Raković, M., Samadi, D., & Vytasek, J. (2019). nStudy: Software for learning analytics about processes for self-regulated learning. Journal of Learning Analytics, 6, 95–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Philip H. Winne.

Additional information

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Winne, P.H. Modeling self-regulated learning as learners doing learning science: How trace data and learning analytics help develop skills for self-regulated learning. Metacognition Learning 17, 773–791 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-022-09305-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-022-09305-y

Keywords

Navigation