Skip to main content
Log in

Counter Cross-Cultural Priming and Relative Deprivation: The Role of Individualism–Collectivism

  • Published:
Social Justice Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper uses cross-cultural comparisons and comparisons obtained by experimental manipulation to examine how cultural and contextual factors influence responses to personal and group relative deprivation. Two studies were conducted, one in an individualistic country (The Netherlands) and one in a collectivistic country (Singapore). One way to examine the influence of the assumed cultural differences in individualism–collectivism is to assign participants to the conditions that elicit “countercultural” psychological states, that is, conditions that prime collectivistic mindsets in the Netherlands and individualistic mindsets in Singapore. Results show that cross-cultural differences have reliable effects on responses to relative deprivation and gratification. Furthermore, findings in the countercultural (experimental) conditions meaningfully differed from those observed in the control conditions in which participants were exposed to neutral stimulus materials. This suggests that cultural mindsets are not fixed, and that countercultural priming can be used to study cross-cultural and contextual differences with high levels of internal validity.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Please note that after the prime manipulation and before reading the scenarios (which included the deprivation and level of treatment manipulations), participants completed the 20 items that, combined, constitute the PANAS (Watson et al., 1988). The PANAS items asked participants to indicate their present affective states. Importantly, after having read the scenarios (and hence having completed all manipulations), participants completed different questions. Specifically, participants then were asked to answer 24 questions about the situation portrayed in the scenarios (see Van Veldhuizen, 2013). We included the PANAS in our studies because earlier research shows that priming manipulations that use open-ended questions to induce mindsets have stronger effects when followed by filler tasks. Thus, as in earlier studies (e.g., Greenberg et al., 1997; Loseman et al., 2009; Van den Bos, 2001; Van den Bos et al., 2012, 2013; Van den Bos & Miedema, 2000; Van Prooijen et al., 2002), the PANAS served as a filler task in our studies and (as mentioned in Footnotes 2 and 4) controlling for positive and negative affective states as assessed by the PANAS did not alter the findings reported. Therefore, the effects of affective states are not reported further. Data and stimulus materials are available on request.

  2. Inspecting Cook’s (1977) distance measure in this analysis (Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2003) revealed that 7 of the 239 participants of Study 1 (2.9 % of the sample) showed a distance score of more than 3 SDs above the mean. These participants were excluded from the main analyses of Study 1. Two other participants (0.8 % of the sample) were also excluded because of missing values on the fairness judgments scale, leaving a total of 230 participants, with 27–30 participants in each cell of our design. Although the answers of at least some of the excluded participants may have deviated from what was expected, including them in our analyses yielded a similar three-way interaction effect between countercultural priming, deprivation, and level of treatment on participants’ fairness judgments as is reported in the main text, although in this case the effect was marginally significant only, F(1, 229) = 3.44, p = .065, η 2 = .01. It also should be noted that when controlling for positive and negative affective states as assessed by the PANAS, the predicted three-way interaction was still statistically significant, F(1, 189) = 4.58, p < .04, η 2 = .02. Therefore, the effects of affective states are not reported further in Study 1.

  3. One participant had a missing value on one of the voice items, explaining the degrees of freedom reported.

  4. Inspecting Cook’s distance measure in this analysis revealed that 7 of the 229 participants of Study 2 (3.1 % of the sample) indicated a distance score of more than 3 SDs above the mean. These participants were excluded from the main analyses of Study 2, leaving a total of 222 participants, with 26–29 participants in each cell of the design. Including the outlying participants in our analyses yielded a significant three-way interaction effect between countercultural priming, deprivation, and level of treatment on participants’ intentions to voice their opinions, F(1, 221) = 6.97, p < .01, η 2 = .02, attesting to the robustness of the predicted effect. When controlling for positive and negative affective states, the hypothesized three-way interaction was statistically significant, F(1, 209) = 9.78, p < .01, η 2 = .03, hence the effects of affective states are not reported further in Study 2.

References

  • Baumeister, R. F. (2005). The cultural animal: Human nature, meaning, and social life. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bechtoldt, M. N., Choi, H. S., & Nijstad, B. A. (2012). Individuals in mind, mates by heart: Individualistic self-construal and collective value orientation as predictors of group creativity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 838–844.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bechtoldt, M. N., De Dreu, C. K., Nijstad, B. A., & Choi, H. S. (2010). Motivated information processing, social tuning, and group creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 622–637.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brockner, J. (2003). Unpacking country effects: On the need to operationalize the psychological determinants of cross-national differences. Research in Organizational Behavior, 25, 333–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, R. D. (1977). Detection of influential observations in linear regression. Technometrics, 19, 15–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crosby, F. (1976). A model of egoistical relative deprivation. Psychological Review, 83, 85–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dambrun, M., Taylor, D. M., McDonald, D. A., Crush, J., & Méot, A. (2006). The relative deprivation-gratification continuum and the attitudes of South Africans toward immigrants: A test of the V-curve hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 1032–1044.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ellemers, N. (2001). Social identity and relative deprivation. In I. Walker & H. J. Smith (Eds.), Relative deprivation: Specification, development and integration (pp. 239–264). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., & Pyszczynski, T. (1997). Terror management theory of self-esteem and cultural worldviews: Empirical assessments and conceptual refinements. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 29, pp. 61–139). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guimond, S., & Dambrun, M. (2002). When prosperity breeds intergroup hostility: The effects of relative deprivation and relative gratification on prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 900–912.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heine, S. J., Markus, H. R., Lehman, D. R., & Kitayama, S. (1999). Is there a universal need for positive self-regard? Psychological Review, 106, 766–794.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hofstede Centre. (2013). National culture: Countries. Retrieved December 15, 2014 from http://geert-hofstede.com/countries.html.

  • Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hong, Y. (2009). A dynamic constructivist approach to culture. Moving from describing culture to explaining culture. In R. S. Wyer, C. Chiu, & Y. Hong (Eds.), Understanding culture: Theory, research, and application (pp. 3–23). New York: Taylor & Francis Group.

  • Hong, Y., & Chiu, C. (2001). Toward a paradigm shift: From cultural differences in social cognition to social cognitive mediation of cultural differences. Social Cognition, 19, 118–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hui, C. H., & Triandis, H. C. (1985). Measurement in cross-cultural psychology: A review and comparison of strategies. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 16, 131–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kawakami, K., & Dion, K. (1993). The impact of salient self-identities on relative deprivation and action intentions. European Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 525–540.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitayama, S. (2002). Culture and basic psychological processes: Toward a system view of culture—Comment on Oyserman et al. (2002). Psychological Bulletin, 128, 89–96.

  • Kühnen, U., Hannover, B., & Schubert, B. (2001). The semantic procedural-interface model of the self: the role of self-knowledge for context-dependent versus context-independent modes of thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 397–409.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kühnen, U., & Oyserman, D. (2002). Thinking about the self influences thinking in general: Cognitive consequences of salient self-concept. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 492–499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loseman, A., Miedema, J., Van den Bos, K., & Vermunt, R. (2009). Exploring how people respond to conflicts between self-interest and fairness: The influence of threats to the self on affective reactions to advantageous inequity. Australian Journal of Psychology, 61, 13–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, D. (2003). Cross-cultural research. In S. Davis (Ed.), The handbook of research methods in experimental psychology (pp. 189–208). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, D., & Hee Yoo, S. (2006). Toward a new generation of cross-cultural research. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 234–250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. G. (2002). Bringing culture to basic psychological theory: Beyond individualism and collectivism—Comment on Oyserman et al. (2002). Psychological Bulletin, 128, 97–109.

  • Moscatelli, S., Albarello, F., Prati, F., & Rubini, M. (2014). Badly off or better off than them? The impact of relative deprivation and relative gratification on intergroup discrimination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107, 248–264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oyserman, D. (2011). Culture as situated cognition: Cultural mindsets, cultural fluency, and meaning making. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychology (Vol. 22, pp. 164–214). Oxon, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 3–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oyserman, D., & Lee, S. W. (2007). Priming “culture”: Culture as situated cognition. In S. Kitayama & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (pp. 255–276). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oyserman, D., & Lee, S. W. (2008). Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 311–342.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Runciman, W. G. (1996). Relative deprivation and social justice: A study of attitudes to social inequality in twentieth-century England. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., & Toguchi, Y. (2003). Pancultural self-enhancement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 60–79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, U. (2012). A 21-word solution. Dialogue, 26, 4–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, H. J., & Ortiz, D. J. (2001). Is it just me? The different consequences of personal and group relative deprivation. In I. Walker & H. J. Smith (Eds.), Relative deprivation: Specification, development and integration (pp. 91–115). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, H. J., Pettigrew, T. F., Pippin, G. M., & Bialosiewicz, S. (2012). Relative deprivation: A theoretical and meta-analytic review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16, 203–232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, H. J., Spears, R., & Oyen, M. (1994). “People like us”: The influence of personal deprivation and group membership salience on justice evaluations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 277–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., & Fong, G. T. (2005). Establishing a causal chain: Why experiments are often more effective than mediational analyses in examining psychological processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 845–851.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stouffer, S. A., Suchman, E. A., DeVinney, L. C., Star, S. A., & Williams, R. M. (1949). The American soldier: Adjustment during Army life (Vol. 1). Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trafimow, D., Triandis, H. C., & Goto, S. G. (1991). Some tests of the distinction between the private self and the collective self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 649–655.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Triandis, H. C., McCusker, C., & Hui, C. H. (1990). Multimethod probes of individualism and collectivism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1006–1020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van den Bos, K. (2001). Uncertainty management: The influence of uncertainty salience on reactions to perceived procedural fairness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 931–941.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van den Bos, K., Brockner, J., Stein, J. H., Steiner, D. D., Van Yperen, N. W., & Dekker, D. M. (2010). The psychology of voice and performance capabilities in masculine and feminine cultures and contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 638–648.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van den Bos, K., Brockner, J., Van den Oudenalder, M., Kamble, S. V., & Nasabi, A. (2013). Delineating a method to study cross-cultural differences with experimental control: Tthe voice effect and countercultural contexts regarding power distance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 624–634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van den Bos, K., Buurman, J., De Theije, V., Doosje, B., Loseman, A., Van Laarhoven, D., et al. (2012). On shielding from death as an important yet malleable motive of worldview defense: Christian versus Muslim beliefs modulating the self-threat of mortality salience. Social Cognition, 30, 778–802.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van den Bos, K., & Miedema, J. (2000). Toward understanding why fairness matters: The influence of mortality salience on reactions to procedural fairness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 355–366.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van den Bos, K., Peters, S. L., Bobocel, D. R., & Ybema, J. F. (2006). On preferences and doing the right thing: Satisfaction with advantageous inequity when cognitive processing is limited. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 273–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Prooijen, J.-W., Van den Bos, K., & Wilke, H. A. M. (2002). Procedural justice and status: Status salience as antecedent of the fair process effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1353–1361.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Veldhuizen, T. S. (2013). Cultural and contextual differences pertaining to relative deprivation: Collectivistic and individualistic responses. MSc thesis, Utrecht University.

  • Walker, I. (1999). Effects of personal and group relative deprivation on personal and collective self-esteem. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 2, 365–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walker, I., & Mann, L. (1987). Unemployment, relative deprivation and social protest. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 175–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kees van den Bos.

Additional information

An earlier version of this article was submitted to Utrecht University as Tanja van Veldhuizen’s MSc thesis.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

van den Bos, K., van Veldhuizen, T.S. & Au, A.K.C. Counter Cross-Cultural Priming and Relative Deprivation: The Role of Individualism–Collectivism. Soc Just Res 28, 52–75 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-014-0230-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-014-0230-6

Keywords

Navigation