Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Social Status, Not Gender Alone, Is Implicated in Different Reactions By Women and Men to Social Ostracism

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Williams and Sommer found that ostracized women, but not men, worked harder on a subsequent collective task, speculating that women’s social compensation was motivated by threatened belongingness. The present 2 × 3 design with 180 U.S. women and men replicated this gender gap in work contributions then closed it using two status-manipulations that favored women’s task abilities or the higher education of undergraduates with high school partners. Additional analyses identified three clusters of participants who failed to compensate: only men in the replication control, women scoring low in self-monitoring, and participants who persisted unsuccessfully to resist exclusion. These patterns shift our focus away from gender and threatened belongingness toward control and status as explanations for the original gender difference.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, J., Ridgeway, C. L., & Zelditch, M. (2002). Construction of status and referential structures. Sociological Theory, 20, 157–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, M. P. (1998). But fingerprints don’t lie, eh?: Prevailing gender ideologies and scientific knowledge. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 22, 739–749.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chodorow, N. (1978). The reproduction of mothering. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, E. G., & Lotan, R. A. (1995). Producing equal-status interaction in the heterogeneous classroom. American Educational Research Journal, 32, 99–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Mladinic, A. (1989). Gender stereotypes and attitudes toward women and men. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 15, 543–558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 878–902.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, R. B., & Fatis, M. (1980). Sex differences in self-monitoring. Psychological Reports, 47, 597–598.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gangestad, S. W., & Snyder, M. (2000). Self-monitoring: Appraisal and reappraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 530–555.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and women’s development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (2001). An ambivalent alliance: Hostile and benevolent sexism as complementary justifications for gender inequality. American Psychologist, 56, 109–118.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hare-Mustin, R. T., & Marecek, J. (1988). The meaning of difference: Gender theory, postmodernism, and psychology. American Psychologist, 43, 455–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hogue, M., & Yoder, J. D. (2003). The role of status in producing depressed entitlement in women’s and men’s pay allocations. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 27, 330–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60, 581–592.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, C., & Ford, R. (1986). Dependence, power, legitimacy, and tactical choice. Social Psychology Quarterly, 59, 126–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kasof, J. (1993). Sex bias in the names of stimulus persons. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 140–163.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kerber, L. K. (1986). Some cautionary words for historians. Signs, 11, 304–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LaFrance, M., Paluck, E. L., & Brescoll, V. (2004). Sex changes: A current perspective on the psychology of gender. In A. H. Eagly, A. E. Beall, & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The psychology of gender (2nd ed.). (pp. 328–344). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latané, B., Williams, K., & Harkins, S. (1979). Many hands make light the work: The causes and consequences of social loafing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 822–832.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. B. (1976). Toward a new psychology of women. Boston, MA: Beacon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Philipp, S. F. (1998). Race and gender differences in adolescent peer group approval of leisure activities. Journal of Leisure Research, 30, 214–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reis, H. T., Senchak, M., & Solomon, B. (1985). Sex differences in the intimacy of social interaction: Further examination of potential explanations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1204–1217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ridgeway, C. (1991). The social construction of status value: Gender and other nominal characteristics. Social Forces, 70, 367–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ridgeway, C. L., & Smith-Lovin, L. (1999). The gender system and interaction. Annual Review of Sociology, 25, 191–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ridgeway, C. L., & Walker, H. A. (2001). Status structures. In A. Branaman (Ed.), Self and society (pp. 298–320). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, M., & Gangestad, S. (1986). On the nature of self-monitoring: Matters of assessment, matters of validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 125–139.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52, 613–629.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swim, J. K. (1994). Perceived versus meta-analytic effect sizes: An assessment of the accuracy of gender stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 21–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T. R., & Blader, L. (2002). Autonomous vs. comparative status: Must we be better than others to feel good about ourselves? Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 89, 813–838.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, K. D. (2001). Ostracism: The power of silence. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, K. D., Cheung, C. K. T., & Choi, W. (2000). Cyberostracism: Effects of being ignored over the Internet. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 748–762.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, K. D., Govan, C. L., Croker, V., Tynan, D., Cruickshank, M., & Lam, A. (2002). Investigations into differences between social- and cyberostracism. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6, 65–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, K. D., & Karau, J. (1991). Social loafing and social compensation: The effects of expectations of co-worker performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 570–581.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, K. D., Karau, S. J., & Bourgeois, M. J. (1993). Working on collective tasks: Social loafing and social compensation. In M. A. Hogg & D. Abrams (Eds.), Group motivation: Social psychological perspectives (pp. 130–148). Hertfordshire, England: Harvester Wheatsheaf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, K. D., & Sommer, K. L. (1997). Social ostracism by coworkers: Does rejection lead to loafing or compensation? Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 693–706.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yoder, J. D., & Kahn, A. S. (1993). Working toward an inclusive psychology of women. American Psychologist, 48, 846–850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yoder, J. D., & Kahn, A. S. (2003). Making gender comparisons more meaningful: A call for more attention to social context. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 27, 281–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zadro, L., Williams, K. D., & Richardson, R. (2004). How low can you go? Ostracism by a computer is sufficient to lower self-reported levels of belonging, control, self-esteem, and meaningful existence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 560–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Janice D. Yoder.

Additional information

The authors thank Kipling Williams and Kristin Sommer for sharing their materials including the cyberball game; Mark Snyder for permission to use the self-monitoring scale; and Arnie Kahn for his supportive and helpful comments. We also appreciate the invaluable help of John Bean, Jessica Christopher, John Haller, David Monter, Pamela Ruesch, and Angela Saniat who, like the first author, served as experimenters throughout data collection.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bozin, M.A., Yoder, J.D. Social Status, Not Gender Alone, Is Implicated in Different Reactions By Women and Men to Social Ostracism. Sex Roles 58, 713–720 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-007-9383-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-007-9383-1

Keywords

Navigation