Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Constructing Gender in Chat Groups

  • Published:
Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We investigated the construction of gender in chat groups. Four unacquainted persons chatted in two gender-anonymous conditions and a non-anonymous control condition. In one anonymous condition, the gender focus was made salient. The other groups did not know about the gender focus. All participants had to guess the gender of the others and give reasons for their decisions. Results suggest that (a) overall, 2/3 of gender guesses fit the sex category of the targets, (b) gender anonymity was more comfortable for women, (c) participants used mostly gender-stereotypic cues to infer gender, however, men and women used syntactic, semantic, and pragmatic cues (with different predictive value) to different degrees, (d) conversational behavior varied depending on gender anonymity, and (e) degree of gender salience was irrelevant for the use of gender as an organizing category.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allen, B. (1995). Gender and computer-mediated communication. Sex Roles, 32, 557–563.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F. (1982). A self-presentation view of social phenomena. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 3–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bavelas, J. B., & Chovil, N. (2000). Visible acts of meaning: An integrated message model of language in face-to-face dialogue. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 19, 163–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, J., Connor, T., & Fisek, M. H. (1974). Expectation states theory: A theoretical research program. Cambridge: Winthorp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bhappu, A., Griffith, T., & Northcraft, G. (1997). Media effects and communication bias in diverse groups. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 70, 199–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biernat, M., & Fuegen, K. (2001). Shifting standards and the evaluation of competence: Complexity in gender-based judgment and decision making. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 707–724.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biernat, M., & Manis, M. (1994). Shifting standards and stereotype-based judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 5–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bortz, J. (1993). Statistik für Sozialwissenschaftler (3rd ed.). Heidelberg: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braun, F. (2001). The communication of gender in Turkish. In M. Hellinger & H. Bussmann (Eds.), Gender across languages (Vol. 1, pp. 283–310). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butler, J. (1991). Das Unbehagen der Geschlechter. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crawford, M. (1995). Talking difference: On gender and language. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowston, K., & Kammerer, E. (1998). Communicative style and gender differences in computer-mediated communication. In E. Bosah (Ed.), Cyberghetto or cybertopia? Race, class, and gender on the Internet (pp. 185–203). Westport, CT: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Danet, B. (1998). Text as mask. Gender, play, and performance on the Internet. In S. G. Jones (Ed.), Cybersociety 2.0: Revisiting communication and community (pp. 129–158). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darley, J. M., & Gross, P. H. (1983). A hypothesis-conforming bias in labeling effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 20–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deaux, K., & LaFrance, M. (1998). Gender. In D. Gilbert, S. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (pp. 788–827). Boston: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deaux, K., & Major, B. (1987). Putting gender into context. An interactive model of gender-related behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 369–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennis, A., Kinney, S., & Hung, Y. (1999). Gender effects and the effects of media richness. Small Group Research, 30, 405–437.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doering, N. (1999). Sozialpsychologie des Internet. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dubrowsky, V., Kiesler, S., & Sethna, B. (1991). The equalization phenomenon: Status effects in computer-mediated and face-to-face decision making groups. Human Computer Interaction, 6, 119–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior. A social-role interpretation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychological Review, 109, 573–598.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eckes, T., & Trautner, H. M. (2000). The developmental social psychology of gender. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garfinkel, H. (1967). Studies in ethnomethodology. Englewood Cliff, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1994). Interaktion und Geschlecht. Frankfurt: Fischer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A. (1984). Nonverbal sex difference: Communication accuracy and expressive style. London: Hopkins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., & Carter, J. D. (1999). Gender-stereotype accuracy as an individual difference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 350–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hannah, A., & Murachver, T. (1999). Gender and conversational style as predictors of conversational behavior. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 18, 153–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herring, S. (1996a). Computer-mediated communication. Linguistic, social, and cross-cultural perspectives. Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herring, S. (1996b). Posting in a different voice: Gender and ethics in computer-mediated communication. In C. Ess (Ed.), Philosophical perspectives on computer-mediated communication (pp. 115–142). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrschel, R. (1994). The impact of varying gender composition on group brainstorming performance in a GSS environment. Computers in Human Behavior, 10, 209–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaffe, J. M., Lee, Y., Huang, L., & Oshagan, H. (1995). Gender, pseudonyms, and CMC: Masking identities and bearing souls. Retreived 03/19/1999 from http://research.haifa.ac.il/jmjaffe/genderpseudocmc.html

  • Jessup, L., Connolly, T., & Tansik, D. (1990). Toward a theory of automated group work: The de-individuating effects of anonymity. Small Group Research, 21, 333–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanter, R. M. (1977). Men and women of the corporation. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kessler, S. J., & McKenna, W. (1978). Gender: An ethnomethodological approach. Chicago: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiesler, S., Siegel, J., & McGuire, T. (1984). Social psychological aspects of computer-mediated communication. American Psychologist, 39, 1123–1134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koch, S. (2004). Constructing gender: A lens-model inspired gender communication approach. Sex Roles, 51, 171–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lorber, J. (1994). Paradoxes of gender. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lott, B. (1995). Distancing from women: Interpersonal sexist discrimination. In B. Lott & D. Maluso (Eds.), The social psychology of interpersonal discrimination (pp. 12–49). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marecek, J., Crawford, M., & Popp, D. (2004). On the construction of gender, sex, and sexualities. In A. H. Eagly, A. E. Beall, & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The psychology of gender (2nd ed., pp. 192–216). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H., & Oyserman, D. (1989). Gender and thought. The role of the self-concept. In M. Crawford & M. Gentry (Eds.), Gender and though: Psychological perspectives (pp. 100–127). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGrath, J. E. (1984). Groups, interaction, and performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGrath, J. E., & Hollingshead, A. B. (1993). Putting the “G” back in GSS: Some theoretical issues about dynamic processes in groups with technological enhancements. In L. M. Jessup & J. Valacich (Eds.), Group support systems: New perspectives (pp. 78–96). New York: MacMillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merten, K. (1995). Inhaltsanalyse. Opladen, Germany: Westdeutscher Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merton, R. K. (1948). The self-fulfilling prophecy. Antioch Review, 8, 193–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, B. (2001). “Doing Gender” im Chat. Heidelberg: Unpublished Diplomarbeit.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mulac, A. (1998). The gender-linked language effect. Do language differences really make a difference? In D. Canary & K. Dindia (Eds.), Sex differences and similarities in communication (pp. 127–153). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noller, P., & Fitzpatrick, M. A. (1988). Perspectives on marital interaction. Philadelphia: Multilingual Matters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ridgeway, C. L. (1992). Gender, interaction, and inequality. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ridgeway, C. L. (2001). Gender, status, and leadership. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 637–656.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Savicki, V., Kelley, M., & Oesterreich, E. (1999). Judgments of gender in computer-mediated communication. Computers in Human Behavior, 15, 185–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, M., Tanke, E. D., & Berscheid, E. (1977). Social perception and interpersonal behavior: On the self-fulfilling nature of social stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 656–666.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sproull, L., & Kiesler, S. (1986). Reducing social context cues: Electronic mail in organizational communication. Management Sciences, 32, 1492–1513.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swann, W. B., Jr. (1983). Self-verification: Bringing social reality into harmony with the self. In J. Suls & A. Greenwald (Eds.), Psychological perspectives of the self (Vol. 2, pp. 33–669). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swim, J. K. (1994). Perceived versus meta-analytic effect sizes: An assessment of the accuracy of gender stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 21–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thimm, C., Rademacher, U., & Kruse, L. (1994). Power-related talk: Control in verbal interaction. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 14, 328–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomson, R., & Murachver, T. (2001). Predicting gender from electronic discourse. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 193–208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turkle, S. (1998). Leben im Nets: Identitaet in Zeiten des Internet. Reinbek, Germany: Rowohlt.

    Google Scholar 

  • West, C., & Zimmerman, D. H. (1987). Doing gender. Gender & Society, 1, 125–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zumbach, J. (2002). Weiterbildung online. In C. Thimm (Ed.), Unternehmenskommunikation offline/online (pp. 260–288). Frankfurt: Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sabine C. Koch.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Koch, S.C., Mueller, B., Kruse, L. et al. Constructing Gender in Chat Groups. Sex Roles 53, 29–41 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-005-4276-7

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-005-4276-7

Key Words

Navigation