Skip to main content
Log in

Goal and DOM datives

  • Published:
Natural Language & Linguistic Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In a range of Indo-European languages (Romance, Albanian, Iranian, Indo-Aryan), the same oblique case (‘dative’) is associated with indirect objects and with animate/definite direct objects, independently of the particular morphology employed to spell out the oblique (inflectional or pre/postpositional). We argue that there is a syntactic category dative coinciding with the morphological one and encompassing both goal dative and definiteness/animacy dative. We provide a characterization of goal dative as an elementary predicate introducing a part-whole (i.e. possession) relation, arguing that the definiteness/animacy dative is an instance of this elementary predicate. Evidence sometimes used against the unification proposed (e.g. passives, agreement) admits of, or requires, other explanations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Other formal morphology frameworks adopting Late Insertion aim to correct some of these problems, for instance nanosyntax has no underspecification (hence Impoverishment). However, it has other problems—for instance an exponent in DM resembles a conventional lexical item (up to underspecification), with a non-contradictory content; an exponent in nanosyntax is a set of all of the specifications it can in principle fill, including potentially contradictory ones. In other words, nanosyntax is much less restrictive than DM in this respect. As far as we can tell, nanosyntactic treatments of case (Caha 2009) go no further than the empirical results of DM.

    In present terms, the lexicon is the locus of externalization in the sense of Berwick and Chomsky (2011) pairing syntactico-semantic content with phonological content. As in Chomsky (1995), furthermore, syntax does not exist but as the product of the merger of lexical items. In what follows we shall often use the expression ‘x lexicalizes y’. In the absence of Late Insertion, ‘x lexicalizes y’ cannot, of course, mean that the phonological string x lexicalizes the syntactic node y, which is what is more usually rendered in DM as ‘x is an exponent for y’. What we mean is that lexical item x lexicalizes concept y, by pairing y with a phonological form z.

  2. In fact, exactly as we claim that goal and DOM datives correspond to the same syntactic category, so it is tempting to see a potential unification between experiencer ‘subject’ datives and the dative/oblique external arguments found in ergativity splits. See also Sect. 3 below for the relevance of the notion of ‘possession’ to goal datives and DOM—the historical and typological literature on Iranian languages (e.g. Montaut 2004) insists on the relevance of the same notion for the assignment of oblique to perfective (‘ergative’) subjects (cf. Manzini et al. 2015).

  3. Other often quoted work on case, for instance Pesetsky and Torrego (2001, 2004) is conceptually close to Chomsky’s (2001, 2008); case is a form of agreement (T agreement for them, where T must be abstract enough to encompass nominative, accusative and oblique). See Sect. 3 for more on the ‘tense/aspect’ generative literature on case, specifically Svenonius (2002).

  4. However in Kashmiri, DOM is found only in progressive tenses. For instance, in the perfect in (i), where the external argument takes the ergative form tsye, the 1st person internal argument takes the bi non-oblique form.

    1. (i)
      figure g
  5. Note that syntactic haplology provides a possible treatment for the incompatibility of ergative case and DOM case in Kashmiri (fn. 4). If in Kashmiri, there is a single oblique per \(v\mathrm{P}\), this determines the impossibility of DOM datives in ergative environments like (i) of fn. 4.

  6. It is worth noting that in some Indo-Aryan languages, the same morphology -ne lexicalizes both the external argument in ergativity splits and the DOM internal argument, leading to double oblique patterns similar to Vafsi (9). An example in point is Bangru (Stroński 2009:246)/Haryanvi (Butt 2007:18), as in (i)–(ii).

    1. (i)
      figure j
    1. (ii)
      figure k

    Vice versa, Iranian languages with three cases are also known. In Yazgulyam, a South-Eastern Iranian language, subjects of intransitive clauses (iii), and subjects and direct objects of transitive clauses (iv) in the perfect receive three morphologically distinct cases in 1st/2nd person, namely absolutive, oblique and accusative respectively.

    1. (iii)
      figure l
    1. (iv)
      figure m
  7. The dative-genitive syncretism characterizes also Iranian languages, Albanian, Greek, as well as Middle Indo-Aryan (cf. Breunis 1990). In Iranian and in Albanian (as well as in Romanian, though not in the particular example we have chosen in (20b)), genitive embedding requires a pre-genitival introducer, namely the ezafe of Iranian varieties, the article of Albanian or Greek etc. (Larson and Yamakido 2008 on Persian; Manzini and Savoia 2011a, 2011b on Albanian; Franco et al. 2015 on the comparison of Iranian and Albanian).

  8. Note that (26a)–(27a) do not violate principle C. The issue of the exact definition of binding principle is orthogonal to those discussed here.

  9. We inserted this discussion at the prompting of an anonymous reviewer. We will return to (⊇) in Sect. 3.2.1.

    A further twist is introduced by the Appl literature, since Cuervo (2003) for Spanish and Diaconescu and Rivero (2007) for Romanian argue that these Romance languages have a Dative Shift alternation. According to this literature the pattern in Italian (26) is observed in the absence of clitic doubling in Spanish and Romanian—while in the presence of clitic doubling pattern, (27) is observed. The overall conclusion is that the dative clitic is the head of an ApplP taking the theme as its object and the Possessor as its Spec and yielding a Dative Shift configuration; in its absence a non-Dative Shifted configuration is obtained (DP-to-DP). On the Appl idea applied to Romance, see fn. 11.

  10. Again the discussion of English was prompted by an anonymous reviewer. Actually the quantifier binding facts reported even by Larson (1988:338), are not completely clear-cut, cf. (i), leaving some room to believe that English to and Romance \(a\) may have the same status after all.

    (i)  ?I gave/sent his paycheck to every worker

  11. In the Appl literature, a dative that takes an event as its argument is a ‘high’ Appl. We refer the reader to Boneh and Nash (2012) for a review of problems arising in classifying Romance datives according to the categories ‘low’ and ‘high’ Appl. We will return to a core instance of ‘high’ Appl, i.e. experiencer datives, in Sect. 4.3.

  12. It is addressed at the prompting of an anonymous reviewer.

  13. Baker and Vinokurova (2010) provide an account for the optionality in Sakha (39), which is however at odds with the absence of semantic contrasts in Italian (40).

  14. Torrego (1998) includes the agentivity of the subject, the telicity of the predicate and the affectedness of the object among the necessary by-products of what she calls the marked accusative. According to Torrego, all of these, as well as the animacy/definiteness of the marked accusative, depend on movement of the internal argument to [Spec, \(v\)]. However the various properties are asserted of the [Spec, \(v\)] position, rather than truly explained by it. Furthermore, the data of von Heusinger and Kaiser (2011) undermine the idea that agentivity (causer) or affectedness (change-of-state) are necessary to determine DOM.

  15. It is also tempting to extend the same treatment to the other main type of oblique case, namely genitive. In (i) ‘the students’ introduced by the preposition di ‘of’ is an experiencer, i.e. it is construed as a possessor of the mental state of ‘fear’. However in (ii) ‘the professor’, also introduced by ‘of’, far from being a possessor of the same mental state, is its subject matter—hence in present terms ‘the professor is (part of) the fear’ of the students. The experiencer and the subject matter can also combine, as in (iii). The contrast between (i) and (ii) is reminiscent of the literature on Romance di or English of as a copula admitting both of direct and inverse construal (Hoekstra and Mulder 1990; den Dikken 1995)—though at the moment we are unable to evaluate the relation between the two proposals.

    (i)

    la paura degli studenti quando sono interrogati

     

    the fear of the students when they.are questioned

    (ii)

    la paura del professore quando interroga

     

    the fear of the professor when he.questions

    (iii)

    la paura del professore degli studenti

     

    the fear of the professor of the students

  16. We are discussing this at the suggestion of an anonymous reviewer.

  17. As an anonymous reviewer points out, we predict that animate definite DPs (and even more so pronouns) will nevertheless be associated with DOM. As far as we can tell this is true, as in Spanish (i).

    1. (i)
      figure ak
  18. We leave it completely open here whether accounting for the relevant cuts requires OT devices like harmonic alignments (Aissen 2003). We note that such devices do not sit well with minimalist theories.

  19. Manzini and Savoia (2011a, 2011b) treat in this way the syncretism of oblique and locative (‘ablative’) case in Albanian.

  20. This treatment of experiencers as possessors of mental states/events is essentially the same as Landau’s (2010), for whom experiencers are locations of mental events. We already commented in Sect. 3.2 on the relation between the present notion of inclusion/part-whole and the notion of location—with which much literature identifies possession.

  21. As briefly discussed for Vafsi in (9)–(10), a Tense/Aspect triggered ergativity split, leading to the oblique realization of the external argument, also characterizes selected Iranian languages. In some of these languages, an internal argument bearing DOM dative/oblique case agrees with the perfect participle, for instance in Baluchi (i). In other languages, it does not. For instance Mâsâli is reported to use a fossilised 3sg agreement morpheme in the perfect, where the internal argument is a DOM oblique, as in (ii).

    1. (i)
      figure av
    1. (ii)
      figure aw

References

  • Ahmed, Tafseer. 2006. Spatial, temporal and structural usages of Urdu ko. In Lexical Functional Grammar (LFG) 2006, eds. Miriam Butt and Tracy H. King. Stanford: CSLI Publications. http://cslipublications.stanford.edu/site/ONLN.shtml. Accessed 03 February 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aissen, Judith. 2003. Differential Object Marking: Iconicity vs. economy. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 21(3): 435–483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alexiadou, Artemis, and Spiridoula Varlokosta. 2007. On the structure and matching effects of free relatives in Greek. In Studies in the morpho-syntax of Greek, ed. Artemis Alexiadou, 222–248. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alvar, Manuel. 1996. Manual de Dialectologia Hispanica: El Espanol de America. Barcelona: Editorial Ariel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arkadiev, Peter M. 2009. Differential argument marking in two-term case systems and its implications for the general theory of case marking. In Differential subject marking, eds. Helen de Hoop and Peter de Swart, 151–171. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, Mark, and Nadya Vinokurova. 2010. Two modalities of Case assignment: Case in Sakha. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 28(3): 593–642.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Béjar, Susana, and Milan Rezac. 2009. Cyclic agree. Linguistic Inquiry 40(1): 35–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Belvin, Robert, and Marcel den Dikken. 1997. There, happens, to, be, have. Lingua 101(3–4): 151–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, Sigrid, and Kyle Johnson. 2004. Double objects again. Linguistic Inquiry 35(1): 97–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berwick, Robert, and Noam Chomsky. 2011. The biolinguistic program: The current state of its evolution and development. In The biolinguistic enterprise, eds. Anna Maria Di Sciullo and Cedric Boeckx, 19–41. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boneh, Nora, and Ivy Sichel. 2010. Deconstructing possession. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 28(1): 1–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boneh, Nora, and Léa Nash. 2012. Core and non-core datives in French. In Variation in datives, eds. Beatriz Fernández and Ricardo Etxepare, 22–49. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bossong, Georg. 1985. Differentielle Objektmarkierung in den Neuiranischen Sprachen. Tübingen: Gunter Narr.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breunis, Andries. 1990. The nominal sentence in Sanskrit and Middle Indo-Aryan. Leiden: Brill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brody, Michael. 2003. Towards an elegant syntax. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burzio, Luigi. 1986. Italian syntax. Dordrecht: Reidel.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Butt, Miriam. 2007. The dative-ergative connection. In Empirical issues in syntax and semantics 6, eds. Olivier Bonami and Patricia Cabredo Hofherr, 69–92. http://www.cssp.cnrs.fr/eiss6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cagri, Ilhan. 2007. Persian accusative case: A lexicalist approach. Hand-out of a talk given at Second International Conference on Iranian Linguistics (ICIL2), August 17–19, 2007. Germany: University of Hamburg.

  • Caha, Pavel. 2009. The nanosyntax of case. Ph.D. Dissertation. CASTL, Tromsø.

  • Calabrese, Andrea. 1998. Some remarks on the Latin case system and its development in Romance. In Theoretical advances on Romance Languages, eds. José Lema and Esthela Trevino, 71–126. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Calabrese, Andrea. 2008. On absolute and contextual syncretism. In The bases of inflectional identity, eds. Andrew Nevins and Asaf Bachrach, 156–205. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cennamo, Michela. 2011. Impersonal constructions and accusative subjects in Late Latin. In Impersonal constructions, eds. Andrej Malchukov and Anna Siewierska, 169–188. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chierchia, Gennaro. 2004. A semantics for unaccusatives and its syntactic consequences. In The unaccusativity puzzle: Explorations of the syntax-lexicon interface, eds. Artemis Alexiadou, Elena Anagnostopoulou, and Martin Everaert, 22–59. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1981. Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1995. The minimalist program. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 2001. Derivation by Phase. In Ken Hale: A life in language, ed. Michael Kenstowicz, 1–54. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 2008. On phases. In Foundational issues in linguistic theory: Essays in honor of Jean-Roget Vergnaud, eds. Robert Freidin, Carlos P. Otero, and Maria Luisa Zubizarreta, 133–167. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 2013. Problems of projection. Lingua 130: 3349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Comrie, Bernard. 1977. In defense of spontaneous demotion: The impersonal passive. In Syntax and semantics 8: Grammatical relations, eds. Peter Cole and Jerrold Sadock, 47–58. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cuervo, María Cristina. 2003. Datives at large. Ph.D. Dissertation. MIT.

  • D’Alessandro, Roberta, and Ian Roberts. 2010. Past participle agreement in Abruzzese: Split auxiliary selection and the null-subject parameter. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 28(1): 41–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Caro, Gerardo. 2008. Remarks on alignment variation in Mâsâli (Southern Tâleši). Ms. SOAS, London.

  • DeLancey, Scott. 1981. An interpretation of split ergativity and related patterns. Language 57(3): 626–657.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diaconescu, Constanţa Rodica, and Maria Luisa Rivero. 2007. An applicative analysis of double object constructions in Romanian. Probus 19(2): 209–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • den Dikken, Marcel. 1995. Particles: On the syntax of verb-particle, triadic, and causative constructions. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dimitrova-Vulchanova, Mila, and Giuliana Giusti. 1998. Fragments of Balkan nominal structure. In Possessors, predicates and movement in the determiner phrase, eds. Artemis Alexiadou and Chris Wilder, 333–360. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, R. M. W. 1979. Ergativity. Language 55(1): 59–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dobrovie-Sorin, Carmen. 1994. The syntax of Romanian. Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dowty, David R. 1991. Thematic proto-roles and argument selection. Language 67(3): 547–619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dum-Tragut, Jasmine. 2009. Armenian: Modern Eastern Armenian. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Enç, Mürvet. 1991. The semantics of specificity. Linguistic Inquiry 22(1): 1–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fillmore, Charles J. 1968. The case for case. In Universals in linguistic theory, eds. Emmon Bach and Robert T. Harms, 1–88. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, Danny. 2002. Antecedent contained deletion and the copy theory of movement. Linguistic Inquiry 33(1): 63–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franco, Ludovico. 2012. On case and clauses: Subordination and the spell-out of non-terminals. In Central European Conference in Linguistics for Postgraduate Students (CECIL’S), eds. Balázs Suranyi and Diana Varga, Vol. 1, 82–103. Budapest: Pázmány Péter Catholic University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franco, Ludovico, M. Rita Manzini, and Leonardo M. Savoia. 2015. Linkers and agreement. The Linguistic Review 32(2): 277–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeze, Ray. 1992. Existentials and other locatives. Language 68(3): 553–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ghomeshi, Jila. 2003. Plural marking, indefiniteness and the noun phrase. Studia Linguistica 57(1): 47–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbertson, George W. 1923. The Balochi language. A grammar and manual. Hertford: Stephen Austin and Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giorgi, Alessandra. 1986. The proper notion of c-command and the binding theory: Evidence from NPs. In North East Linguistics Society (NELS), Vol. 6. Amherst: GLSA, University of Massachusetts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giusti, Giuliana. 2002. The functional structure of noun phrases: A bare phrase structure approach. In Functional structure in DP and IP, ed. Guglielmo Cinque, 54–90. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haig, Geoffrey. 2008. Alignment change in Iranian languages: A construction grammar approach. Berlin: De Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hale, Kenneth, and Samuel Jay Keyser. 1993. On argument structure and the lexical expression of grammatical relations. In The view from building 20. Essays in linguistics in honor of Sylvain Bromberger, eds. Kenneth Hale and Samuel Jay Keyser, 53–109. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harley, Heidi. 2002. Possession and the Double Object Construction. Linguistic Variation Yearbook 2: 29–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, James. 1994. The syntax-phonology mapping in Catalan and Spanish clitics. In MITWPL 21: Papers on phonology and morphology, eds. Andrew Carnie and Heidi Harley, 321–353. Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Heusinger, Klaus. 2008. Verbal semantics and the diachronic development of Differential Object Marking in Spanish. Probus 20(1): 1–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Heusinger, Klaus, and Georg A. Kaiser. 2011. Affectedness and differential object marking in Spanish. Morphology 21(3–4): 593–617.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoekstra, Teun, and Rene Mulder. 1990. Unergatives as copular verbs: Locational and existential predication. The Linguistic Review 7(1): 1–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hook, Peter Edwin, and Omkar N. Koul. 2004. Case as agreement: Non-nominative subjects in Eastern Shina, non-dative objects in Kashmiri and Poguli, and labile subjects in Kashmiri and Gujarati intransitive inceptives. In Non-nominative subjects, eds. Peri Bhaskararao and Karumuri Venkata Subbarao, Vol. 1, 213–225. Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, Richard. 1992. So-called ‘double objects’ and grammatical relations. Language 68(2): 251–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iemmolo, Giorgio. 2010. Topicality and differential object marking: Evidence from Romance and beyond. Studies in Languages 34(2): 239–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaeggli, Osvaldo. 1981. Topics in Romance syntax. Dordrecht: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karimi, Simin. 2003. On object positions, specificity and scrambling in Persian. In Word order and scrambling, ed. Simin Karimi, 91–124. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Karimi, Simin. 2005. A minimalist approach to scrambling: Evidence from Persian. Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kayne, Richard. 1984. Connectedness and binary branching. Dordrecht: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kayne, Richard. 1989. Facets of Romance Past Participle Agreement. In Dialect variation and the theory of grammar, ed. Paola Benincà, 85–103. Dordrecht: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kayne, Richard. 1994. The antisymmetry of syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Khokhlova, Ludmila. 2002. Syntactic peculiarities of Rajasthani. Paper presented at the 17th European Conference on Modern South Asian Studies (ECMSAS), Heidelberg, September 2002.

  • Kiparsky, Paul. 1998. Partitive case and aspect. In Projecting from the lexicon, eds. Miriam Butt and Wilhelm Geuder, 265–307. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiparsky, Paul. 2001. Structural case in Finnish. Lingua 111(4–7): 315–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Korn, Agnes. 2008. Marking of Arguments in Balochi Ergative and Mixed Constructions. In Aspects of Iranian linguistics, eds. Simin Karimi, Vida Samiian, and Donald Stilo, 249–276. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kornfilt, Jaklin. 1997. Turkish grammar. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kratzer, Angelika. 2009. Making a pronoun. Fake indexicals as windows into the properties of pronouns. Linguistic Inquiry 40(2): 187–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landau, Idan. 2010. The locative syntax of experiencers. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larson, Richard. 1988. On the double object construction. Linguistic Inquiry 19(3): 335–392.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larson, Richard, and Hiroko Yamakido. 2008. Ezafe and the deep position of nominal modifiers. In Adjectives and adverbs. Syntax, semantics, and discourse, eds. Louise McNally and Christopher Kennedy, 43–70. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazard, Gilbert. 1984. Actance variations and categories of the object. In Objects: Towards a theory of grammatical relations, ed. Frans Plank, 249–289. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leonetti, Manuel. 2004. Specificity and Differential Object Marking in Spanish. Catalan Journal of Linguistics 3: 75–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, Lisa. 2011. Possessive WITH in Germanic: HAVE and the role of P. Syntax 14(4): 355–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loporcaro, Michele. 2008. Opposizioni di caso nel pronome personale: I dialetti del mezzogiorno in prospettiva romanza. In I dialetti meridionali tra arcaismo e interferenza, ed. Alessandro De Angelis, 207–235. Palermo: Centro di Studi filologici e linguistici siciliani.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malchukov, Andrej. 2008. Animacy and asymmetries in differential case marking. Lingua 118(2): 203–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manzini, M. Rita, and Leonardo M. Savoia. 2005. I dialetti italiani e romanci: Morfosintassi generativa. Alessandria: Edizioni dell’Orso, 3 vols.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manzini, M. Rita, and Leonardo M. Savoia. 2007. A unification of morphology and syntax. Studies in Romance and Albanian varieties. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manzini, M. Rita, and Leonardo M. Savoia. 2011a. Grammatical categories. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Manzini, M. Rita, and Leonardo M. Savoia. 2011b. Reducing ‘case’ to denotational primitives: Nominal inflections in Albanian. Linguistic Variation 11(1): 76–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manzini, M. Rita, and Leonardo M. Savoia. 2014. From Latin to Romance: Case loss and preservation in pronominal systems. Probus 26(2): 217–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manzini, M. Rita, Leonardo M. Savoia, and Ludovico Franco. 2015. Ergative case, Aspect and Person splits: Two case studies. Acta Linguistica Hungarica 62(3).

  • Marantz, Alec. 2000 [1991]. Case and licensing. In Arguments and case, ed. Eric Reuland, 11–30. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Originally published as: Marantz, Alec. 1991. Case and licensing. In German Westphal, Benjamin Ao, and Hee-Rahk Chae (eds.), Eastern States Conference on Linguistics (ESCOL) 8, 234–253. Cornell, Ithaca: Cornell Linguistics Club.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • McFadden, Thomas. 2006. German inherent datives and argument structure. In Datives and other cases: Between argument structure and event structure, eds. Daniel Hole, André Meinunger, and Werner Abraham, 49–77. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Mohanan, Tara. 1994. Case OCP in Hindi. In Theoretical perspectives on word order issues in South Asian languages, eds. Miriam Butt, Tracy H. King, and Gillian Ramchand, 185–215. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mohanan, Tara. 1995. Woodhood and lexicality: Noun incorporation in Hindi. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 13(1): 75–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Montaut, Annie. 2004. Oblique Main Arguments in Hindi/Urdu as localizing predications. Questioning the Category of Subject. In Non nominative subjects, eds. Peri Bhaskararao and K. V. Subbarao, Vol. 2, 33–56. Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Neeleman, Ad, and Hans van de Koot. 2006. Syntactic haplology. In The Blackwell companion to syntax, eds. Martin Everaert and Henk Van Riemsdijk, Vol. IV, 684–710. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ormazabal, Javier, and Juan Romero. 2007. The Object Agreement Constraint. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 25(2): 315–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Payne, John. 1980. The decay of ergativity in Pamir languages. Lingua 51(2–3): 147–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perlmutter, David, and Paul Postal. 1983. Towards a universal characterization of passivization. In Studies in relational grammar, ed. David Perlmutter, Vol. 1, 3–29. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pesetsky, David. 1995. Zero syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pesetsky, David, and Esther Torrego. 2001. T-to-C Movement: Causes and consequences. In Ken Hale: A life in language, ed. Michael Kenstowicz, 355–426. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pesetsky, David, and Esther Torrego. 2004. Tense, case, and the nature of syntactic categories. In The syntax of time, eds. Jacqueline Gueron and Jacqueline Lecarme, 495–537. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plank, Frans. 1985. The extended accusative/restricted nominative in perspective. In Relational typology, ed. Frans Plank, 269–310. Berlin: De Gruyter.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Pylkkänen, Liina. 2008. Introducing arguments. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Reinhart, Tanya. 1997. Syntactic effects of lexical operations: Reflexives and unaccusatives. OTS working papers in linguistics. Utrecht: University of Utrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards, Norvin. 2010. Uttering trees. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • van Riemsdijk, Henk. 2008. Identity Avoidance: OCP-effects in Swiss Relatives. In Foundational issues in linguistic theory: Essays in honor of Jean-Roget Vergnaud, eds. Robert Freidin, Carlos P. Otero, and Maria Luisa Zubizarreta, 227–250. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sportiche, Dominique. 1996. Clitic constructions. In Phrase structure and the lexicon, eds. Johan Rooryck and Laurie Zaring, 213–276. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Stilo, Donald. 2004. Grammar notes. In Vafsi folk tales, ed. Donald Stilo, 223–244. Wiesbaden: Reichert.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stilo, Donald. 2010. Ditransitive constructions in Vafsi. A corpus based study. In Studies in ditransitive constructions: A comparative handbook, eds. Andrej Malchukov, Martin Haspelmath, and Bernard Comrie, 243–276. Berlin: De Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stroński, Krzysztof. 2009. Variation of ergativity patterns in Indo-Aryan. Poznań Studies in Contemporary Linguistics 45(2): 237–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suñer, Margarita. 1988. The role of agreement in clitic-doubled constructions. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 6(3): 391–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Svenonius, Peter. 2002. Icelandic case and the structure of events. Journal of Comparative Germanic Linguistics 5(1–3): 197–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tenny, Carol. 1994. Aspectual roles and the syntax-semantics interface. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Torrego, Esther. 1998. The dependencies of objects. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verbeke, Saartje. 2013. Alignment and ergativity in new Indo-Aryan languages. Berlin: De Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wali, Kashi, and Omkar N. Koul. 1997. Kashmiri: A cognitive-descriptive grammar. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yip, Moira. 1998. Identity avoidance in phonology and morphology. In Morphology and its relation to phonology and syntax, eds. Steven G. Lapointe, Diane K. Brentari, and Patrick M. Farrell, 216–246. Stanford: CSLI.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank the anonymous reviewers and our editor, Ad Neeleman, for their help in shaping this work. Many of the ideas in this article originate from our collaboration with Leonardo Savoia, who we think of as our third coauthor; all mistakes are of course ours. Rita Manzini’s research was partially supported through a PRIN 2012 grant from the Italian Ministry of Education, Universities and Research (MIUR), Research title: Theory, Experimentation, Applications: Long distance dependencies. Ludovico Franco was supported by a PRIN 2010/11 grant to Dr. Benedetta Baldi at the University of Florence, and by a Portuguese FCT grant (IF/00846/2013). The work was conceived jointly; for Italian administrative purposes Ludovico Franco takes responsibility for Sects. 1 (excluding 1.1), 2.1, 4.2, 4.4 (excluding 4.4.1).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to M. Rita Manzini.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Manzini, M.R., Franco, L. Goal and DOM datives. Nat Lang Linguist Theory 34, 197–240 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-015-9303-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-015-9303-y

Keywords

Navigation