Skip to main content
Log in

When motivation backfires: Optimal levels of motivation as a function of cognitive capacity in information relevance perception and social judgment

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Motivation and cognitive capacity are central variables in major models of social judgment and persuasion. However, the exact nature of their interplay in judgment processes has remained ambiguous. The present paper reports on two experimental studies tackling this issue. In Study 1 we demonstrated a cross-over pattern of means for the interaction effect of motivation and cognitive capacity on judgment, with high motivation being beneficial under high cognitive capacity but detrimental under low cognitive capacity. This effect was explained by the participants’ subjective perception of information relevance. In Study 2, the role of information relevance was further investigated, showing that for highly motivated participants, judgment was differently affected by information with low versus high relevance when cognitive capacity was high but not when it was low. In the discussion, we elaborate on these effects, advocating a more dynamic perspective on (social) judgment, acknowledging the conditional (opposite) effects of motivation, in function of cognitive capacity.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In contrast, directional motivation refers to particular instances in which the individual prefers a particular judgment and assigns differential weight to information according to this preference. This specific type of motivation is not the focus of the present study.

  2. In this regard, Sigall and Mills (1998) argued that even a nonsignificant result for a manipulation check does not necessarily invalidate significant effects of the manipulation on the dependent variables.

  3. Note that ANOVA with repeated measures of judgment at T1 and T2 yields results that are identical to ANOVA using the change measure. We used the latter procedure to facilitate communication of the results.

  4. For levels of motivation and cognitive capacity being high–high, high–low, low–high and low–low: M = 10.68, SD = 5.28; M = 15.47, SD = 3.99; M = 10.33, SD = 4.41; M = 13.00, SD = 5.16, respectively.

  5. Analyses including the outlier data yielded similar results; F(1, 70) = 11.25, p = .001, F(1, 70) = 1.61, p = .21, and F(1, 70) = 0.91, p = .34, for the main effect of capacity, motivation, and their interaction respectively.

  6. Because in Study 1, the number of consulted pieces of information did not mediate the effects of the independent variables, this variable was no longer of central interest in Study 2. However to ensure comparability in the procedure, also in Study 2, information was presented upon the participants request, with a maximum of 10 pieces.

  7. In the Pelham and Neter (1995) studies, a clear (but incorrect) heuristic rule was available to complete the task. In the present tasks, it is hard to identify such a heuristic rule or heuristic information that could be straightforwardly used to reach judgment. For example, all pieces of information were of equal length and stemmed from the same undisclosed source in Study 2, whereas in Study 1, participants received information from various sources but their message all pointed into the same direction.

  8. This perspective is also reminiscent of the Yerkes-Dodson law (Yerkes and Dodson 1908), stating that too high levels of arousal can degrade performance. Moreover, the impact of motivation has been described to follow the same inverted U-curve and various studies have relied on the Yerkes-Dodson law to explain counterproductive effects of accountability (as used in Study 1) or incentives (as used in Study 2) on performance (see, e.g., Ashton 1990; Mobbs et al. 2009).

References

  • Ashton, R. H. (1990). Pressure and performance in accounting decision settings: Paradoxical effects of incentives, feedback, and justification. Journal of Accounting Research, 28, 148–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Tal, Y., Kishon-Rabin, L., & Tabak, N. (1997). The effect of need and ability to achieve cognitive structuring on cognitive structuring. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1158–1176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Betsch, T., Plessner, H., Schwieren, C., & Gütig, R. (2001). I like it but I don’t know why: A value-account approach to implicit attitude formation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 242–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brehm, J. W., & Self, E. (1989). The intensity of motivation. In M. R. Rozenweig & L. W. Porter (Eds.), Annual review of psychology (pp. 109–131). Palo Alto: Annual Reviews, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chaiken, S., Liberman, A., & Eagly, A. H. (1989). Heuristic and systematic information processing within and beyond the persuasion context. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 12–252). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich College Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erb, H.-P., Kruglanski, A. W., Chun, W. Y., Pierro, A., Mannetti, L., & Spiegel, S. (2003). Searching for commonalities in human judgement: The parametric unimodel and its dual mode alternatives. European Review of Social Psychology, 14, 1–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., & Richter, M. (2010). Effort mobilization when the self is involved: Some lessons from the cardiovascular system. Review of General Psychology, 14, 212–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hockey, G. R. J. (1997). Compensatory control in the regulation of human performance under stress and high workload. A cognitive-energetical framework. Biological Psychology, 45, 73–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janis, I. L., & Mann, L. (1977). Decision-making: A psychological analysis of conflict, choice, and commitment. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, M. F., & Miller, L. E. (1978). Reducing the effects of juror bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1443–1455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kassin, S. M., & Hochreich, D. J. (1977). Instructional set: A neglected variable in attribution research? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 3, 620–623.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., & Freund, T. (1983). The freezing and un-freezing of lay-inferences: Effects of impressional primacy, ethnic stereotyping and numerical anchoring. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 448–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., & Gigerenzer, G. (2010). Intuitive and deliberative judgments are based on common processes. Unpublished manuscript: University of Maryland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., & Mayseless, O. (1988). Contextual effects in hypothesis testing: The role of competing alternatives and epistemic motivations. Social Cognition, 6, 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., & Webster, D. M. (1996). Motivated closing of the mind: “Seizing” and “Freezing”. Psychological Review, 103, 263–283.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Erb, H. P., Pierro, A., Mannetti, L., & Chun, W. Y. (2006). On parametric continuities in the world of binary either ors. Psychological Inquiry, 17, 153–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Pierro, A., Mannetti, L., Erb, H. P., & Chun, W. Y. (2007). On the parameters of human judgment. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 255–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Dechesne, M., Orehek, E., & Pierro, A. (2009). Three decades of lay epistemics: The why, how, and who of knowledge formation. European Review of Social Psychology, 20, 146–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Belanger, J. J., Chen, X. Y., Kopetz, C., Pierro, A., & Mannetti, L. (2012). The energetics of motivated cognition: A force-Field Analysis. Psychological Review, 119, 1–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loersch, C., McCaslin, M. J., & Petty, R. E. (2011). Exploring the impact of social judgeability concerns on the interplay of associative and deliberative attitude processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 1029–1032.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mobbs, D., Hassabis, D., Seymour, B., Marchant, J. L., Weiskopf, N., Dolan, R. J., et al. (2009). Choking on the money: Reward-based performance decrements are associated with midbrain activity. Psychological Science, 20, 955–962.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pelham, B. W., & Neter, E. (1995). The effect of motivation of judgment depends, on the difficulty of the judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 581–594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E. (1994). Two routes to persuasion: State of the art. In G. d’Ydewalle, P. Eelen, & P. Berteleson (Eds.), International Perspectives on Psychological Science (Vol. 2, pp. 229–247). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.

  • Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication and persuation: Central and peripheral routes to attitude change. New York: Springer.

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in simple and multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roets, A., & Van Hiel, A. (2007). Separating ability from need: Clarifying the dimensional structure of the need for closure scale. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 266–280.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roets, A., Van Hiel, A., Cornelis, I., & Soetens, B. (2008). Determinants of performance and invested effort. A need for closure by cognitive capacity interaction analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 779–792.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sigall, H., & Mills, J. (1998). Measures of independent variables and mediators are useful in social psychology experiments: But are they necessary? Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 218–226.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., & Fong, G. T. (2005). Establishing a causal chain: Why experiments are often more effective than mediational analyses in examining psychological processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 845–851.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tan, H. T., Ng, B. P., & Mak, W. Y. (2002). The effects of task complexity on auditors’ performance: The impact of accountability and knowledge. Auditing: A Journal of Practice and Theory, 21, 81–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tetlock, P. E. (1985). Accountability: A social check on the fundamental attribution error. Social Psychology Quarterly, 48, 227–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, R. A., & Kirby, L. D. (2001). Effort determination of cardiovascular response: An integrative analysis with applications in social psychology. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 255–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yerkes, R. M., & Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology of Psychology, 18, 459–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Martin F. Kaplan for providing access to the materials from Kaplan and Miller (1978). This research was supported by a post-doctoral research grant from the National Fund for Scientific Research-Flanders (Belgium) awarded to the first author.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Arne Roets.

Appendix

Appendix

See Table 1.

Table 1 Means and standard deviations for judgments at T1 and T2 in Study 1 and Study 2

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Roets, A., Van Hiel, A. & Kruglanski, A.W. When motivation backfires: Optimal levels of motivation as a function of cognitive capacity in information relevance perception and social judgment. Motiv Emot 37, 261–273 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-012-9299-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-012-9299-0

Keywords

Navigation