Skip to main content
Log in

Spelling in Adults: The Combined Influences of Language Skills and Reading Experience

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

One hundred and twelve university students completed 7 tests assessing word-reading accuracy, print exposure, phonological sensitivity, phonological coding and knowledge of English morphology as predictors of spelling accuracy. Together the tests accounted for 71% of the variance in spelling, with phonological skills and morphological knowledge emerging as strong predictors of spelling accuracy for words with both regular and irregular sound-spelling correspondences. The pattern of relationships was consistent with a model in which, as a function of the learning opportunities that are provided by reading experience, phonological skills promote the learning of individual word orthographies and structural relationships among words.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allyn F.A., Burt J.S. (1998). Pinch my wig or winch my pig: Spelling, spoonerisms, and other language skills. Reading and Writing, 10:51–74

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen R.H., Piepenbrock R., Gulikers L. (1995). The CELEX lexical database (Release 2) [CD-ROM]. Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA

    Google Scholar 

  • Barry C. (1994). Spelling routes (or roots or rutes). In: Brown G.D.A., Ellis N.C. (eds) Handbook of spelling: Theory, process and intervention. John Wiley & Sons, Chichester England, pp. 27–49

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley L., Bryant P. (1985). Rhyme and reason in reading and spelling. The University of Michigan Press, Ann Arbor Michigan

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown G.D.A., Loosemore R.P.W. (1994). Computational approaches to normal and impaired spelling. In: Brown G.D.A., Ellis N.C. (eds) Handbook of spelling: Theory, process and intervention. John Wiley, Chichester, England, pp. 319–335

    Google Scholar 

  • Burt J.S., Butterworth P. (1996). Spelling in adults: Orthographic transparency, learning new letter strings, and reading accuracy. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 8:3–43

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burt J.S., Fury M.B. (2000). Spelling in adults: The role of reading skills and experience. Reading and Writing 13:1–30

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burt J.S., Shrubsole C. (2000). Processing of phonological representations and adult spelling proficiency. Australian Journal of Psychology 52:100–109

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burt J.S., Tate H. (2002). Does a reading lexicon provide orthographic representations for spelling?. Journal of Memory and Language 46:518–543

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell R., Butterworth B. (1985). Phonological dyslexia and dysgraphia in a highly literate subject: A developmental case with associated deficits in phonemic processing and awareness. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 37:435–475

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Caravolas M., Hulme C., Snowling M.J. (2001). The foundations of spelling ability: Evidence from a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Memory & Language 45:751–774

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle J.F. (1988). Knowledge of derivational morphology and spelling ability in fourth, sixth, and eighth graders. Applied Psycholinguistics 9:247–266

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carter R.E., Bradshaw J.L. (1984). Producing “Spoonerisms” on demand: Lexical, phonological and orthographic factors in a new experimental paradigm. Speech Communication 3:347–360

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castles A., Coltheart M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition 47:149–180

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chateau D., Jared D. (2000). Exposure to print and word recognition process. Memory & Cognition 28(1):143–153

    Google Scholar 

  • Chliounaki K., Bryant P. (2002). Construction and learning to spell. Cognitive Development 17(3–4):1489–1499

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart M., Davelaar E., Jonasson J.T., Besner D. (1977). Access to the internal lexicon. In: Dornic S. (eds) Attention and performance VI. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 535–555

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart M., Rastle K., Perry C., Langdon R., Ziegler J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review 108:204–256

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cormier P., Dea S. (1997). Distinctive patterns of relationship of phonological awareness and working memory with reading development. Reading and Writing 9:193–206

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham A.E., Perry K.E., Stanovich K.E., Share D.L. (2002). Orthographic learning during reading: examining the role of self-teaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 82:185–199

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham A.E., Stanovich K.E. (1993). Children’s literacy environments and early word recognition subskills. Reading and Writing 5(2):193–204

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon M., Stuart M., Masterson J. (2002). The relationship between phonological awareness and the development of orthographic representations. Reading and Writing 15:295–316

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drake D.A., Ehri L.C. (1984). Spelling acquisition: Effects of pronouncing words on memory for their spellings. Cognition and Instruction 1:297–320

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman L.B., Rueckl J., DiLiberto K., Pastizzo M., Vellutino F.R. (2002). Morphological analysis by child readers as revealed by the fragment completion task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 9(3):529–535

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer F.W., Shankweiler D., Liberman I.Y. (1985). Spelling proficiency and sensitivity to word structure. Journal of Memory and Language 24:423–441

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foorman B.R., Francis D.J., Fletcher J.M., Lynn A. (1996). Relation of phonological and orthographic processing to early reading: Comparing two approaches to regression-based, reading-level-match designs. Journal of Educational Psychology 88:639–652

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frith U. (1980a). Unexpected spelling problems. In: Frith U. (eds) Cognitive processes in spelling. Academic Press, London England, pp. 495–515

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith U. (eds) (1980b). Cognitive processes in spelling. Academic Press, London England

    Google Scholar 

  • Holmes V.M., Carruthers J. (1998). The relation between reading and spelling in skilled adult readers. Journal of Memory and Language 39:264–289

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes V.M., Ng E. (1993). Word-specific knowledge, word-recognition strategies, and spelling ability. Journal of Memory and Language 32:230–257

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Houghton G., Zorzi M. (2003). Normal and impaired spelling in a connectionist dual-route architecture. Cognitive Neuropsychology 20:115–162

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jastak S., Wilkinson G.S. (1984). Wide range achievement test (revised). Jastak Associates, Inc., Wilmington, Delaware

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston R.S. (1993). The role of memory in learning to read, write and spell: A review of recent research. In: Davies G.M., Logie R.H. (eds) Memory in everyday life. Elsevier Science Publishers, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, pp. 59–77

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jorm A.F., Schoknecht C. (1981). Role of visual word recognition checks in children’s spelling. Australian Journal of Psychology 33:393–403

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kreiner D.S. (1992). Reaction time measures of spelling: Testing a two-strategy model of skilled spelling. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 18:765–776

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kreiner D.S., Gough P.B. (1990). Two ideas about spelling: Rules and word-specific memory. Journal of Memory and Language 29:103–118

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyster S.-A.H. (2002). The effects of morphological versus phonological awareness training in kindergarten on reading development. Reading & Writing 15(3–4):261–294

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nunes T., Bryant P., Bindman M. (1997). Learning to spell regular and irregular verbs. Reading and Writing 9(5–6):427–449

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ormrod J.E. (1985). Visual memory in a spelling matching task: Comparison of good and poor spellers. Perceptual and Motor Skills 61:183–188

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ormrod J.E., Cochran K.F. (1988). Relationship of verbal ability and working memory to spelling achievement and learning to spell. Reading Research and Instruction 28(1):33–43

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennington B.F., Lefly D.L., van Orden G.C., Bookman M.O., Smith S.D. (1987). Is phonology bypassed in normal or dyslexic development?. Annals of Dyslexia 37:62–89

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., & Leonardelli, G. J. (2001). Calculation for the Sobel test: An interactive tool for mediation tests. Retrieved 30 May, 2004, from http://www.unc.edu/preacher/sobel/sobel.htm

  • Raven J.C. (1984). Advanced progressive matrices (Set II). Oxford Psychologists Press, Oxford, UK

    Google Scholar 

  • Share D.L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition 55(2):151–218

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Share D.L. (1999). Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of the self-teaching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 72(2):95–129

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Share D.L. (2004). Orthographic learning at a glance: On the time course and developmental onset of self-teaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 87:267–298

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich K.E., Cunningham A.E. (1992). Studying the consequences of literacy within a literate society: The cognitive correlates of print exposure. Memory and Cognition 20:51–68

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich K.E., West R.F. (1989). Exposure to print and orthographic processing. Reading Research Quarterly 21:402–433

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman R., Baron J. (1983). Phonemic-analysis training helps children benefit from spelling sound rules. Memory & Cognition 11:382–389

    Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer W.E., Nesdale A.R. (1985). Phonemic segmentation skill and beginning reading. Journal of Educational Psychology 77:417–427

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino F.R. (1994). The increasingly inextricable relationship between orthographic and phonological coding in learning to read: Some reservations about current methods of operationalizing orthographic coding. In: Berninger V.W. (eds) The varieties of orthographic knowledge, 2: Relationships to phonology, reading, and writing. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dordrecht, pp. 47–111

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino F.R., Scanlon D.M., Tanzman M.S. (1994). Components of reading ability: Issues and problems in operationalizing word identification, phonological coding, and orthographic coding. In: Lyon G.R. (eds) Frames of reference for the assessment of learning disabilities: New views on measurement issues. Paul H. Brookes Publishing Company, Baltimore, MD, USA, pp. 279–332

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jennifer S. Burt.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Burt, J.S. Spelling in Adults: The Combined Influences of Language Skills and Reading Experience. J Psycholinguist Res 35, 447–470 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-006-9024-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-006-9024-9

Keywords

Navigation