Skip to main content
Log in

Mother and Child Facial Expression Labeling Skill Relates to Mutual Responsiveness During Emotional Conversations

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The capacity to engage with one’s child in a reciprocally responsive way is an important element of successful and rewarding parent–child conversations, which are common contexts for emotion socialization. The degree to which a parent–child dyad shows a mutually responsive orientation presumably depends on both individuals’ socio-emotional skills. For example, one or both members of a dyad needs to be able to accurately interpret and respond to the other’s nonverbal cues, such as facial expressions, to facilitate mutually responsive interactions. Little research, however, has examined whether and how mother and/or child facial expression decoding skill relates to dyads’ emotional mutuality during conversations. We thus examined associations between both mother and child facial expression decoding skill and observed emotional mutuality during parent-preschooler conversations about happy child memories. Results lend support to our hypotheses by suggesting that both mother and child capacities to read others’ emotional cues make distinct contributions to parent–child emotional mutuality in the context of reminiscing conversations. Specifically, mothers’ accurate decoding of child facial expressions predicted maternal displays of positive affect and interest, while children’s accurate decoding of adult facial expressions predicted dyadic displays of mutual enjoyment. Contrary to our hypotheses, however, parent/child facial expression decoding skills did not interact to predict observed mutual responsiveness. These findings underscore the importance of attending to both parent and child contributions to successful dyadic interactions that facilitate effective emotion socialization.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bakeman, R., & Brownlee, J. R. (1980). The strategic use of parallel play: A sequential analysis. Child Development, 51(3), 873–878. doi:10.2307/1129476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bakeman, R., & Gottman, J. M. (1997). Observing interaction: An introduction to sequential analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bavelas, J., & Chovil, N. (2000). Visible acts of meaning: An integrated message model of language in face-to-face dialogue. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 19(2), 163–194. doi:10.1177/0261927X00019002001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bretherton, I. (1993). From dialogue to internal working models: The co-construction of self in relationships. In C. A. Nelson (Ed.), Memory and affect in development (pp. 237–263). Hillsdale, NJ England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broth, M. R. (2004). Associations between mothers’ negative emotionality and stress and their socialization of emotion practices: Mothers’ emotional competence as resiliency or risk (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Atlanta, Georgia: Emory University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, R. (1985). The parent–child early relational assessment instrument and manual. Madison, WI: Department of Psychiatry, University of Wisconsin Medical School.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, R. (1999). The parent–child early relational assessment: A factorial validity study. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 59, 821–846. doi:10.1177/00131649921970161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, K. E., & Ladd, G. W. (2000). Connectedness and autonomy support in parent–child relationships: Links to children’s socioemotional orientation and peer relationships. Developmental Psychology, 36(4), 485–498. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.36.4.485.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dailey, R. M. (2008). Assessing the contribution of nonverbal behaviors in displays of confirmation during parent-adolescent interactions: An actor-partner interdependence model. Journal of Family Communication, 8(1), 62–91. doi:10.1080/15267430701573599.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denham, S. A., Blair, K. A., DeMulder, E., Levitas, J., Sawyer, K., Auerbach-Major, S., & Queenan, P. (2003). Preschool emotional competence: Pathway to social competence. Child Development, 74(1), 238–256. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00533.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dubois-Comtois, K., Cyr, C., & Moss, E. (2011). Attachment behavior and mother–child conversations as predictors of attachment representations in middle childhood: A longitudinal study. Attachment and Human Development, 13(4), 335–357. doi:10.1080/14616734.2011.584455.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, J. (1987). The beginnings of moral understanding: Development in the second year. In J. Kagan & S. Lamb (Eds.), The emergence of morality in young children. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody picture vocabulary test-revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ensor, R., Spencer, D., & Hughes, C. (2011). ‘You feel sad?’ emotion understanding mediates effects of verbal ability and mother–child mutuality on prosocial behaviors: Findings from 2 years to 4 years. Social Development, 20(1), 93–110. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2009.00572.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, R., Greenbaum, C. W., & Yirmiya, N. (1999). Mother–infant affect synchrony as an antecedent of the emergence of self-control. Developmental Psychology, 35(1), 223–231. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.1.223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, R., Magori-Cohen, R., Galili, G., Singer, M., & Louzoun, Y. (2011). Mother and infant coordinate heart rhythms through episodes of interaction synchrony. Infant Behavior and Development, 34(4), 569–577. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.06.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feng, X., Shaw, D. S., Skuban, E. M., & Lane, T. (2007). Emotional exchange in mother–child dyads: Stability, mutual influence, and associations with maternal depression and child problem behavior. Journal of Family Psychology, 21(4), 714–725. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.21.4.714.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Field, T., Healy, B. T., Goldstein, S., & Guthertz, M. (1990). Behavior-state matching and synchrony in mother-infant interactions of nondepressed versus depressed dyads. Developmental Psychology, 26(1), 7–14. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.26.1.7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fivush, R. (2007). Maternal reminiscing style and children’s developing understanding of self and emotion. Clinical Social Work Journal, 35(1), 37–46. doi:10.1007/s10615-006-0065-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fivush, R., & Vasudeva, A. (2002). Remembering to relate: Socioemotional correlates of mother–child reminiscing. Journal of Cognition and Development, 3(1), 73–90. doi:10.1207/S15327647JCD0301_5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fivush, R., & Wang, Q. (2005). Emotion talk in mother–child conversations of the shared past: The effects of culture, gender, and event valence. Journal of Cognition and Development, 6(4), 489–506. doi:10.1207/s15327647jcd0604_3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, H. S., & Riggio, R. E. (1981). Effect of individual differences in nonverbal expressiveness on transmission of emotion. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 6(2), 96–104. doi:10.1007/BF00987285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garner, P. W., & Waajid, B. (2008). The associations of emotion knowledge and teacher-child relationships to preschool children’s school-related developmental competence. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 29(2), 89–100. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2007.12.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harrist, A. W., & Waugh, R. M. (2002). Dyadic synchrony: Its structure and function in children’s development. Developmental Review, 22(4), 555–592. doi:10.1016/S0273-2297(02)00500-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Healey, D. M., Gopin, C. B., Grossman, B. R., Campbell, S. B., & Halperin, J. M. (2010). Mother–child dyadic synchrony is associated with better functioning in hyperactive/inattentive preschool children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 1058–1066. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02220.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holler, J., & Wilkin, K. (2011). Co-speech gesture mimicry in the process of collaborative referring during face-to-face dialogue. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 35(2), 133–153. doi:10.1007/s10919-011-0105-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kochanska, G. (1997). Mutually responsive orientation between mothers and their young children: Implications for early socialization. Child Development, 68(1), 94–112. doi:10.2307/1131928.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kochanska, G. (2002). Mutually responsive orientation between mothers and their young children: A context for the early development of conscience. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(6), 191–195. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kochanska, G., Aksan, N., Penney, S. J., & Doobay, A. F. (2007). Early positive emotionality as a heterogenous trait: Implications for children’s self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(6), 1054–1066. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.6.1054.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuebli, J., Butler, S., & Fivush, R. (1995). Mother–child talk about past emotions: Relations of maternal language and child gender over time. Cognition and Emotion, 9(2–3), 265–283. doi:10.1080/02699939508409011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LaFrenière, P. J., Dumas, J. E., Capuano, F., & Dubeau, D. (1992). Development and validation of the Preschool Socioaffective Profile. Psychological Assessment, 4(4), 442–450. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.4.4.442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laible, D., & Song, J. (2006). Constructing emotional and relational understanding: The role of affect and mother-child discourse. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 52(1), 44–69. doi:10.1353/mpq.2006.0006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Letzring, T. D. (2008). The good judge of personality: Characteristics, behaviors, and observer accuracy. Journal of Research in Personality, 42(4), 914–932. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2007.12.003.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E. (1983). Social-emotional development and response to stressors. In N. Garmezy & M. Rutter (Eds.), Stress, coping, and development in children (pp. 217–234). Baltimore, MD US: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E. (1992). The role of parents in the socialization of children: An historical overview. Developmental Psychology, 28(6), 1006–1017. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.28.6.1006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, S. R., Chorney, J. M., Cohen, L. L., & Kain, Z. N. (2013). Sequential analysis of mothers’ and fathers’ reassurance and children’s postoperative distress. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 38(10), 1121–1129. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jst061.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Michalik, N. M., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Ladd, B., Thompson, M., & Valiente, C. (2007). Longitudinal relations among parental emotional expressivity and sympathy and prosocial behavior in adolescence. Social Development, 16(2), 286–309. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00385.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, L. C., Lechner, R. E., & Rugs, D. (1985). Development of conversational responsiveness: Preschoolers’ use of responsive listener cues and relevant comments. Developmental Psychology, 21(3), 473–480. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.21.3.473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mize, J., & Pettit, G. S. (1997). Mothers’ social coaching, mother–child relationship style, and children’s peer competence: Is the medium the message? Child Development, 68(2), 312–332. doi:10.2307/1131852.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nowicki, S., & Duke, M. P. (1994). Individual differences in the nonverbal communication of affect: The diagnostic analysis of nonverbal accuracy scale. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 18(1), 9–35. doi:10.1007/BF02169077.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parker, A. E., Mathis, E. T., & Kupersmidt, J. B. (2013). How is this child feeling? Preschool-aged children’s ability to recognize emotion in faces and body poses. Early Education and Development, 24(2), 188–211. doi:10.1080/10409289.2012.657536.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Pianta, R. C., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (1989). Continuity and discontinuity in maternal sensitivity at 6, 24, and 42 months in a high-risk sample. Child Development, 60(2), 481–487. doi:10.2307/1130992.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raikes, H., & Thompson, R. A. (2008). Conversations about emotion in high-risk dyads. Attachment and Human Development, 10(4), 359–377. doi:10.1080/14616730802461367.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raver, C. C., Garner, P. W., & Smith-Donald, R. (2007). The roles of emotion regulation and emotion knowledge for children’s academic readiness: Are the links causal? In R. C. Pianta, M. J. Cox, K. L. Snow, R. C. Pianta, M. J. Cox, & K. L. Snow (Eds.), School readiness and the transition to kindergarten in the era of accountability (pp. 121–147). Baltimore, MD, US: Paul H Brookes Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenblatt, J. S. (1965). The basis of synchrony in the behavioral interaction between the mother and her offspring in the laboratory rat. In B. M. Foss (Ed.), Determinants of infant behavior (pp. 3–45). London: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sales, J. M., Fivush, R., & Peterson, C. (2003). Parental reminiscing about positive and negative events. Journal of Cognition and Development, 4(2), 185–209. doi:10.1207/S15327647JCD0402_03.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skuban, E., Shaw, D. S., Gardner, F., Supplee, L. H., & Nichols, S. R. (2006). The correlates of dyadic synchrony in high-risk, low-income toddler boys. Infant Behavior and Development, 29(3), 423–434. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.02.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass, S. E. (1985). Women’s intuition: The effect of subordinate role on interpersonal sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(1), 146–155. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.49.1.146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass, S. E., Hecht, M. A., & Ploutz-Snyder, R. (1998). Interpersonal sensitivity: Expressivity or perceptivity?. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(1), 238–249. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.238.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass, S. E., & Rosenthal, R. (1985). Interpersonal sensitivity and skills in decoding nonverbal channels: The value of face value. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 6(3), 243–255. doi:10.1207/s15324834basp0603_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trees, A. R. (2000). Nonverbal communication and the support process: Interactional sensitivity in interactions between mothers and young adult children. Communication Monographs, 67(3), 239–261. doi:10.1080/03637750009376509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trentacosta, C. J., & Izard, C. E. (2007). Kindergarten children’s emotion competence as a predictor of their academic competence in first grade. Emotion, 7(1), 77–88. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Emotions and emotional communication in infants. American Psychologist, 44(2), 112–119. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.44.2.112.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Bergen, P., & Salmon, K. (2010). The association between parent–child reminiscing and children’s emotion knowledge. New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 39(1), 51–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zlochower, A. J., & Cohn, J. F. (1996). Vocal timing in face-to-face interaction of clinically depressed and nondepressed mothers and their 4-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 19(3), 371–374. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(96)90035-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Erin B. Tone.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tolleson, A.M., Tone, E.B., Schroth, E.A. et al. Mother and Child Facial Expression Labeling Skill Relates to Mutual Responsiveness During Emotional Conversations. J Nonverbal Behav 40, 205–219 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-016-0232-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-016-0232-1

Keywords

Navigation