Skip to main content
Log in

Evidence for a cognitive bias of interpretation toward threat in individuals with a Type D personality

  • Published:
Journal of Behavioral Medicine Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Biological and behavioral mediators link Type D personality with a poor prognosis in heart disease. However, the mediator role of cognitive biases is still unknown. This study tested whether Type D individuals exhibit an interpretative bias for ambiguous social situations. For this aim we examined Type D and non-Type D individuals’ evaluations of written social situations that varied in terms of situations’ clarity (clear, ambiguous) and social judgment (neutral, negative). A convenience sample of 42 young, healthy adults rated each situation in relation to the difficulty of formulating a verbal response, anticipated distress, and perceived threat, and they completed the Type D personality scale (DS14; Denollet, 2005). Results showed an interpretation bias among Type D individuals, as they rated ambiguous or neutral situations as significantly more distressing compared to non-Type D individuals. Only clearly negative situations were rated similarly by Type D and non-Type D individuals. The discussion suggests that this interpretation bias in Type D individuals would increase their vulnerability to perceived stress.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In ambiguous situations Type D individuals reported greater difficulties in formulating a verbal response and more anticipated distress than non-Type D individuals (respectively F(1, 41) = 10.80, p = .002, partial eta2 = .213; F(1, 41) = 22.37, P < .001, partial eta2 = .359). In clear situation these effects were respectively F(1,41) = 3.68, p = .06, partial eta2 = .084; F(1,41) = 10.90, p = .002, partial eta2 = .214).

References

  • Amir, N., Beard, C., & Bower, E. (2005). Interpretation bias and social anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29, 433–443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amir, N., Foa, E. B., & Coles, M. E. (1998). Automatic activation and strategic avoidance of threat-relevant information in social phobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 285–290.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Amir, N., Weber, G., Beard, C., Bomyea, J., & Taylor, C. (2008). The effect of a single session attention modification program on response to a public speaking challenge in socially anxious individuals. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 860–868.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barefoot, J. C., Patterson, J. C., Haney, T. L., Cayton, T. G., Hickman, J. R., & Williams, R. B. (1994). Hostility in asymptomatic men with angiographically confirmed coronary-artery disease. American Journal of Cardiology, 74, 439–442.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bartko, J. J. (1966). The intraclass correlation coefficient as a measure of reliability. Psychological Reports, 19, 3–11.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Beard, C., & Amir, N. (2008). A multi-session interpretation modification program: Changes in interpretation and social anxiety symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 1135–1141.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chida, Y., & Steptoe, A. (2009). The association of anger and hostility with future coronary heart disease: A meta-analytic review of prospective evidence. Journal of the American College of Cardiology, 53, 936–946.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. M. (2001). A cognitive perspective on social phobia. In W. R. Crozier & L. E. Alden (Eds.), International handbook of social anxiety: Concepts, research, and interventions relating to the self and shyness (pp. 405–430). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conraads, V. M., Denollet, J., De Clerck, L. S., Stevens, W. J., Bridts, C., & Vrints, C. J. (2006). Type D personality is associated with increased levels of tumour necrosis factor (TNF)-alpha and TNF-alpha receptors in chronic heart failure. International Journal of Cardiology, 113, 34–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dembroski, T. M., MacDougall, J. M., Costa, P. T., Jr, & Grandits, G. A. (1989). Components of hostility as predictors of sudden death and myocardial infarction in the Multiple Risk Factor Intervention Trial. Psychosomatic Medicine, 51, 514–522.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Denollet, J. (2000). Type D personality—A potential risk factor refined. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 49, 255–266.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Denollet, J. (2005). DS14: Standard assessment of negative affectivity, social inhibition, and Type D personality. Psychosomatic Medicine, 67, 89–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Denollet, J., & Brutsaert, D. L. (1998). Personality, disease severity, and the risk of long-term cardiac events in patients with a decreased ejection fraction after myocardial infarction. Circulation, 97, 167–173.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Denollet, J., Conraads, V. M., Brutsaert, D. L., De Clerck, L. S., Stevens, W. J., & Vrints, C. J. (2003). Cytokines and immune activation in systolic heart failure: The role of Type D personality. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 17, 304–309.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Denollet, J., Pedersen, S. S., Vrints, C. J., & Conraads, V. M. (2006). Usefulness of type D personality in predicting five-year cardiac events above and beyond concurrent symptoms of stress in patients with coronary heart disease. American Journal of Cardiology, 97, 970–973.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Denollet, J., Schiffer, A. A., & Spek, V. (2010). A general propensity to psychological distress affects cardiovascular outcomes: Evidence from research on the Type D (distressed) personality profile. Circulation Cardiovascular Quality Outcomes, 3, 546–557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denollet, J., Sys, S. U., Stroobant, N., Rombouts, H., Gillebert, T. C., & Brutsaert, D. L. (1996). Personality as independent predictor of long-term mortality in patients with coronary heart disease. Lancet, 347, 417–421.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frasure-Smith, N., & Lesperance, F. (2010). Depression and cardiac risk: present status and future directions. Heart, 96, 173–176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heeren, A., Peschard, V., & Philippot, P. (submitted). The causal nature of attentional biases for threatening stimuli in the perception of social rejection.

  • Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K. M., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The trier social stress test—A tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology, 28, 76–81.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kuper, H., Marmot, M., & Hemingway, H. (2002). Systematic review of prospective cohort studies of psychosocial factors in the etiology and prognosis of coronary heart disease. Seminars in Vascular Medicine, 2, 267–314.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martens, E. J., Kupper, N., Pedersen, S. S., Aquarius, A. E. A. M., & Denollet, J. (2007). Type-D personality is a stable taxonomy in post-MI patients over an 18-month period. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 63, 545–550.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, R., Hirsch, C. R., Mathews, A., Smith, K., & Clark, D. M. (2007). Facilitating a benign interpretation bias in a high socially anxious population. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 1517–1529.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson, A., Kuper, H., & Hemingway, H. (2006). Depression as an etiologic and prognostic factor in coronary heart disease: a meta-analysis of 6362 events among 146 538 participants in 54 observational studies. European Heart Journal, 27, 2763–2774.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Dell, K., Masters, K. S., Spielmans, G. I., & Maisto, S. A. (in press). Does Type-D Personality predict outcomes among patients with cardiovascular disease? A meta-analytic review. Journal of Psychosomatic Research.

  • Paulsen, N., Callan, V. J., Grice, T. A., Rooney, D., Gallois, C., Jones, E., et al. (2005). Job uncertainty and personal control during downsizing: A comparison of survivors and victims. Human Relations, 58, 463–496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rozanski, J., Juraszynski, Z., Kusmierczyk, M., & Sitko, T. (1999). Repair of coarctation of the aorta with simultaneous coronary artery bypass grafting without cardiopulmonary bypass. European Journal of Cardio-Thoracic Surgery, 15, 536–538.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schiffer, A. A., Denollet, J., Widdershoven, J. W., Hendriks, E. H., & Smith, O. R. F. (2007). Failure to consult for symptoms of heart failure in patients with a Type D personality. Heart, 93, 814–818.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, N. B., Richey, J. A., Buckner, J. D., & Timpano, K. R. (2009). Attention training for generalized social anxiety disorder. Journal Abnormal Psychology, 118, 5–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 420–428.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stansfeld, S. A., Fuhrer, R., Shipley, M. J., & Marmot, M. G. (2002). Psychological distress as a risk factor for coronary heart disease in the Whitehall II Study. International Journal of Epidemiology, 31, 248–255.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Melle, J., de Jonge, P., Spijkerman, T., Tijssen, J. G. P., Ormel, J., van Veldhuisen, D. J., et al. (2004). Prognostic association of depression following myocardial infarction with mortality and cardiovascular events: A meta-analysis. Psychosomatic Medicine, 66, 814–822.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Voncken, M. J., Bogels, S. M., & de Vries, K. (2003). Interpretation and judgmental biases in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 1481–1488.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by grants 1.1.233.09 from the Belgian National Funds for Scientific Research (FNRS-FRS) to Delphine Grynberg (Research Fellow). This study was supported by grants 1.5.175.06 and 1.5.148.10 from the Belgian National Fund for Scientific Research granted to Olivier Luminet (Senior Research associate). This research was also supported by a Joined Research Grant (ARC 06/11-337) from the Belgian French Community to Olivier Luminet, and by a VICI grant (#453-04-004) from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research to Johan Denollet. We thank Betty Chang for her help in improving the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Delphine Grynberg.

Appendix

Appendix

Clear negative

  1. 1.

    You have been dismissed for a serious offence.

  2. 2.

    You explain to your colleagues that you are late for the meeting because your train was twenty minutes late. They tell you that it is unacceptable and ask you to not participate anymore in these meetings.

  3. 3.

    You receive a mail from a friend, who tells you that he was very disappointed by your aggressive behavior at his party, and that he does not want to see you ever again.

Ambiguous negative

  1. 1.

    You go to a birthday party, and some people look at each other and start whispering when they see you come in.

  2. 2.

    You explain to your colleagues that you are late for the meeting because your train was twenty minutes late. They listen to your explanation and then take a coffee break.

  3. 3.

    You are going to a club, and the doorman refuses to let you in.

  4. 4.

    Someone enters in your classroom and firmly declares that the headmaster wants to see you immediately.

Clear neutral

  1. 1.

    You are in the train and the ticket inspector asks for your ticket.

  2. 2.

    You explain to your colleagues that you are late for the meeting because your train was twenty minutes late. They tell you that there is no problem.

  3. 3.

    You walk in the street and a person points out to you that your laces are undone.

  4. 4.

    You call a friend to ask him to go to the cinema with other friends, but he tells you that he can not come because he is with his mother, whom he has not seen for a while.

Ambiguous neutral

  1. 1.

    You go to a birthday party and a person you don’t know nods his head to you.

  2. 2.

    You go to the bookshop and the bookseller sells you a newspaper without the TV guide.

  3. 3.

    You pass your colleague in the street and after having looked at you, he starts walking more quickly and then crosses the street.

  4. 4.

    You are going to the cinema and the usherette does not give you the program.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Grynberg, D., Gidron, Y., Denollet, J. et al. Evidence for a cognitive bias of interpretation toward threat in individuals with a Type D personality. J Behav Med 35, 95–102 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-011-9351-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-011-9351-7

Keywords

Navigation