Skip to main content
Log in

Within- and Cross-Modal Integration and Attention in the Autism Spectrum

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although impairment in sensory integration is suggested in the autism spectrum (AS), empirical evidences remain equivocal. We assessed the integration of low-level visual and tactile information within and across modalities in AS and typically developing (TD) individuals. TD individuals demonstrated increased redundancy gain for cross-modal relative to double tactile or visual stimulation, while AS individuals showed similar redundancy gain between cross-modal and double tactile conditions. We further observed that violation of the race model inequality for cross-modal conditions was observed over a wider proportion of the reaction times distribution in TD than AS individuals. Importantly, the reduced cross-modal integration in AS individuals was not related to atypical attentional shift between modalities. We conclude that AS individuals displays selective decrease of cross-modal integration of low-level information.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adolphs, R. (2002). Recognizing emotion from facial expressions: Psychological and neurological mechanisms. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews,1(1), 21–62.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alvarado, J. C., Rowland, B. A., Stanford, T. R., & Stein, B. E. (2008). A neural network model of multisensory integration also accounts for unisensory integration in superior colliculus. Brain Research,1242, 13–23.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Alvarado, J. C., Stanford, T. R., Vaughan, J. W., & Stein, B. E. (2007a). Cortex mediates multisensory but not unisensory integration in superior colliculus. Journal of Neurosciences,27(47), 12775–12786.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alvarado, J. C., Vaughan, J. W., Stanford, T. R., & Stein, B. E. (2007b). Multisensory versus unisensory integration: Contrasting modes in the superior colliculus. Journal of Neurophysiology,97(5), 3193–3205.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Badzakova-Traikov, G., Hamm, J. P., & Waldie, K. E. (2005). The effects of redundant stimuli on visuospatial processing in developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia,43, 473–478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bahrick, L. E. (2010). Intermodal perception and selective attention to intersensory redundancy: Implications for typical social development and autism. In G. Dans & T. D. Wachs (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of infant development (2nd ed.). Oxford, England: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bahrick, L. E., & Todd, J. T. (2011). Multisensory processing in autism spectrum disorders: Intersensory processing disturbance as a basis for atypical development. In M. T. Dans & M. M. Murray (Eds.), Frontiers in the neural basis of multisensory processes. London: FrancisGroup.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baum, S. H., Stevenson, R. A., & Wallace, M. T. (2015). Behavioral, perceptual, and neural alterations in sensory and multisensory function in autism spectrum disorder. Progress in Neurobiology,134, 140–160.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bebko, J. M., Weiss, J. A., Demark, J. L., & Gomez, P. (2006). Discrimination of temporal synchrony in intermodal events by children with autism and children with developmental disabilities without autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,47(1), 88–98.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Behrmann, M., Thomas, C., & Humphreys, K. (2006). Seeing it differently: Visual processing in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,10(6), 258–264.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boer-Schellekens, L., Keetels, M., Eussen, M., & Vroomen, J. (2013). No evidence for impaired multisensory integration of low-level audiovisual stimuli in adolescents and young adults with autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia,51, 3004–3013.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bonneh, Y. S., Belmonte, M. K., Pei, F., Iversen, P. E., Kenet, T., Akshoomoff, N., et al. (2008). Cross-modal extinction in a boy with severely autistic behaviour and high verbal intelligence. Cognitive Neuropsychology,25(5), 635–652.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick, M. (2004). Neuroscience: Probing the neural basis of body ownership. Science,305(5685), 782–783.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brandwein, A. B., Foxe, J. J., Butler, J. S., Frey, H.-P., Bates, J. C., Shulman, L. H., et al. (2015). Neurophysiological indices of atypical auditory processing and multisensory integration are associated with symptom severity in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,45, 230–244.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brandwein, A. B., Foxe, J. J., Butler, J. S., Russo, N. N., Altschuler, T. S., Gomes, H., et al. (2013). The development of multisensory integration in high-functioning autism: High-density electrical mapping and psychophysical measures reveal impairments in the processing of audiovisual inputs. Cerebral Cortex,23, 1329–1341.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brock, J., Brown, C. C., Boucher, J., & Rippon, G. (2002). The temporal binding deficit hypothesis of autism. Developmental Psychopathology,14(2), 209–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cascio, C. J., Foss-Feig, J. H., Burnette, C. P., Heacock, J. L., & Cosby, A. A. (2012). The rubber hand illusion in children with autism spectrum disorders: Delayed influence of combined tactile and visual input on proprioception. Autism,16(4), 406–419.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cascio, C. J., Lorenzi, J., & Baranek, G. T. (2016a). Self-reported pleasantness ratings and examiner-coded defensiveness in response to touch in children with ASD: Effects of stimulus material and bodily location. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder,46(5), 1528–1537.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cascio, C. J., Woynaroski, T., Baranek, G. T., & Wallace, M. T. (2016b). Toward an interdisciplinary approach to understanding sensory function in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research,9, 920–925.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Charbonneau, G., Bertone, A., Lepore, F., Nassim, M., Lassonde, M., Mottron, L., et al. (2013a). Multi-level alterations in the multisensory processing of dynamic emotion expressions in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Neuropsychologia,51, 1002–1010.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charbonneau, G., Véronneau, M., Boudrias-Fournier, C., Lepore, F., & Collignon, O. (2013b). The ventriloquist in periphery: Impact of eccentricity-related reliability on audio-visual localization. Journal of Vision,13(12), 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collignon, O., Charbonneau, G., Peters, F., Nassim, M., Lassonde, M., Lepore, F., et al. (2013). Reduced multisensory facilitation in persons with autism. Cortex, 49(6), 1704–1710.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corballis, M. C. (1998). Interhemispheric neural summation in the absence of the corpus callosum. Brain,121, 1795–1807.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Courchesne, E., Townsend, J., Akshoomoff, N. A., Saitoh, O., Yeung-Courchesne, R., Lincoln, A. J., et al. (1994). Impairment in shifting attention in autistic and cerebellar patients. Behavioral Neuroscience,108, 848–865.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dakin, S., & Frith, U. (2005). Vagaries of visual perception in autism. Neuron,48(3), 497–507.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Webb, S. J., Carver, L., Panagiotides, H., & McPartland, J. (2004). Young children with autism shoe atypical brain responses to fearful versus neutral facial expressions of emotion. Developmental Science,7(3), 340–359.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Heering, A., Dormal, G., Pelland, M., Lewis, T., Maurer, D., & Collignon, O. (2016). A brief period of postnatal visual deprivation alters the balance between auditory and visual attention. Current Biology,26, 3101–3105.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. (2010). The social role of touch in humans and primates: Behavioral function and neurobiological mechanisms. Biobehavioral Reviews,34(2), 260–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ernst, M. O., & Banks, M. S. (2002). Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature,415, 429–433.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Field, T. (2001). Touch. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forster, B., Cavina-Pratesi, C., Aglioti, S. M., & Berlucchi, G. (2002). Redundant target effect and intersensory facilitation from visual-tactile interactions in simple reaction time. Experimental Brain Research,143, 480–487.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foss-Feig, J. H., Kwakye, L. D., Cascio, C. J., Burnette, C. P., Kadivar, H., Stone, W. L., et al. (2010). An extended multisensory temporal binding window in autism spectrum disorders. Experimental Brain Research,203(2), 381–389.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Foxe, J. J., Molhom, S., Del Bene, V. A., Frey, H. P., Russo, N. N., Blanco, D., et al. (2015). Severe multisensory speech integration deficits in high-functioning school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and their resolution during early adolescence. Cerebral Cortex,25(2), 298–312.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U., & Happe, F. (1994). Autism: Beyond “theory of mind”. Cognition,50(1–3), 115–132.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gingras, G., Rowland, B. A., & Stein, B. E. (2009). The differing impact of multisensory and unisensory integration on behavior. The Journal of Neuroscience,29, 4897–4902.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Girard, S., Collignon, O., & Lepore, F. (2011). Multisensory gain within and across hemispaces in simple and choice reaction time paradigms. Experimental Brain Research,214(1), 1–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Girard, S., Pelland, M., Lepore, F., & Collignon, O. (2013). Impact of the spatial congruence of redundant targets on within-modal and cross-modal integration. Experimental Brain Research,224(2), 275–285.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gondan, M., Lange, K., Rösler, F., & Röder, B. (2004). The redundant target effect is affected by modality switch costs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,11, 307–313.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield, K., Ropar, D., Smith, A. D., Carey, M., & Newport, R. (2015). Visuo-tactile integration in autism: Atypical temporal binding may underlie greater reliance on proprioceptive information. Molecular Autism,6, 51.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Harrar, V., Tammam, J., Pérez-Bellido, A., Pitt, A., Stein, J., & Spence, C. (2014). Multisensory integration and attention in developmental dyslexia. Current Biology,24, 531–535.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hershenson, M. (1962). Reaction time as a measure of intersensory facilitation. Journal of Experimental Psychology,63, 289.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hertenstein, M. (2002). Touch: its communicative functions in infancy. Human Development,45(2), 70–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertenstein, M., Keltner, D., App, B., Bulleit, B., & Jaskolla, A. (2006). Touch communicates distinct emotions. Emotion,6(3), 528.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iarocci, G., & Burack, J. A. (2004). Intact covert orienting to peripheral cues among children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,34(3), 257–264.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iarocci, G., & McDonald, J. (2006). Sensory integration and the perceptual experience of persons with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,36(1), 77–90.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Khan, S., Michmizos, K., Tommerdahl, M., Ganesan, S., Kitzbichler, M. G., Zetino, M., et al. (2015). Somatosensory cortex functional connectivity abnormalities in autism show opposite trends, depending on direction and spatial scale. Brain,138, 1394–1409.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kwakye, L. D., Foss-Feig, J. H., Cascio, C. J., Stone, W. L., & Wallace, M. T. (2011). Altered auditory and multisensory temporal processing in autism spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience,4(129), 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landry, R., & Bryson, S. E. (2004). Impaired disengagement of attention in young children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,45, 1115–1122.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laurienti, P. J., Burdette, J. H., Maldjian, J. A., & Wallace, M. T. (2006). Enhanced multisensory integration in older adults. Neurobiology of Aging,27(8), 1155–1163.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leekam, S. R., Lopez, B., & Moore, C. (2000). Attention and joint attention in preschool children with Autism. Developmental Psychology,36(2), 261–273.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Jr., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,30(3), 205–223.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,24(5), 659–685.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Magnée, M. J., de Gelder, B., van Engeland, H., & Kemner, C. (2008). Audiovisual speech integration in pervasive developmental disorder: Evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Child Psychology Psychiatry,49(9), 995–1000.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maitre, N. L., Key, A. P., Chorna, O. D., Slaughter, J. C., Matusz, P. J., Wallace, M. T., et al. (2017). The dual nature of early-life experience on somatosensory processing in the human infant brain. Current Biology,27(7), 1048–1054.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marco, E. J., Hinkley, L. B., Hill, S. S., & Nagarajan, S. S. (2011). Sensory processing in autism: A review of neurophysiological findings. Pediatric Research,69, 48–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miles, E., Brown, R., & Poliakoff, E. (2011). Investigating the nature and time-course of the modality shift effect between vision and touch. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,64(5), 871–888.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. (1982). Divided attention evidence for coactivation with redundant signals. Cognitive Psychology,14, 247–279.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. (1986). Timecourse of coactivation in bimodal divided attention. Perception and Psychophysics,40(5), 331–343.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miniussi, C., Girelli, M., & Marzi, C. (1998). Neural site of the redundant target effect electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,10, 216–230.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mongillo, E. A., Irwin, J. R., Whalen, D. H., Klaiman, C., Carter, A. S., & Schultz, R. T. (2008). Audiovisual processing in children with and without autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,38, 1349–1358.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulières, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,36(1), 27–43.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, M. M., Foxe, J. J., Higgins, B. A., Javitt, D. C., & Schroeder, C. E. (2001). Visuo-spatial neural response interactions in early cortical processing during a simple reaction time task: A high-density electrical mapping study. Neuropsychologia,39, 828–844.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Myers, B. J. (1984). Mother-infant bonding: The status of this critical period hypothesis. Developmental Review,4, 240–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noel, J.-P., De Niear, M. A., Lazzara, N. S., & Wallace, M. T. (2018a). Uncoupling between multisensory temporal function and nonverbal turn-taking in autism spectrum disorder. IEEE Transactions on Cognitive and Developmental Systems,10(4), 973–982.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noel, J. P., De Niear, M. A., Stevenson, R., Alais, D., & Wallace, M. T. (2017). Atypical rapid audio-visual temporal recalibration in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research,10(1), 121–129.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Noel, J. P., Lytle, M., Cascio, C., & Wallace, M. T. (2018b). Disrupted integration of exteroceptive and interoceptive signaling in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research,11(1), 194–205.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Noel, J. P., Stevenson, R. A., & Wallace, M. T. (2018c). Atypical audiovisual temporal function in autism and schizophrenia: Similar phenotype, different cause. European Journal of Neuroscience,47(10), 1230–1241.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Neill, M., & Jones, R. S. P. (1997). Sensory-perceptual abnormalities in autism: A case for more research? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,27, 283–293.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Occelli, V., Esposito, G., Venuti, P., Arduino, G. M., & Zampini, M. (2013). Attentional shifts between audition and vision in autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders,7, 517–525.

    Google Scholar 

  • Otto, T. U., & Mamassian, P. (2012). Noise and correlations in parallel perceptual decision making. Current Biology,22, 1391–1396.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paton, B., Hohwy, J., & Enticott, P. G. (2012). The rubber hand illusion reveals proprioceptive and sensorimotor differences in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,42(9), 1870–1883.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Poole, D., Gowen, E., Warren, P. A., & Poliakoff, E. (2015). Investigating visual-tactile interactions over time and space in adults with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder,45, 3316–3326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poole, D., Gowen, E., Warren, P. A., & Poliakoff, E. (2018). Visual-tactile selective attention in autism spectrum condition: An increased influence of visual distractors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,147, 1309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powers, A. R., Hevey, M. A., & Wallace, M. T. (2012). Neural correlates of multisensory perceptual learning. The Journal of Neurosciences,32(18), 6263–6274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powers, A. R., Hillock-Dunn, A., & Wallace, M. T. (2016). Generalization of multisensory perceptual learning. Scientific Reports,6, 23374.

    Google Scholar 

  • Puts, N. A. J., Wodka, E. L., Harris, A. D., Crocetti, D., Tommerdahl, M., Mostofsky, S. H., et al. (2017). Reduced GABA and altered somatosensory function in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research,10(4), 608–619.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raab, D. H. (1962). Statistical facilitation of simple reaction times. Transactions of the New York Academy of Sciences,24, 574–590.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Renner, P., Klinger, L. G., & Klinger, M. R. (2006). Exogenous and endogenous attention orienting in autism spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychology,12(4–5), 361–382.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reuter Lorenz, P. A., Nozawa, G., Gazzaniga, M. S., & Hughes, H. C. (1995). Fate of neglected targets: a chronometric analysis of rendundant target effects in the bisected brain. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,21, 211–230.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S., & Wehner, E. (2003). Parent reports of sensory symptoms in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,33, 631–642.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schütz-Bosbach, S., Mancini, B., Aglioti, S. M., & Haggard, P. (2006). Self and other in the human motor system. Current Biology,16, 1830–1834.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schütz-Bosbach, S., Musil, J. J., & Haggard, P. (2009). Touchant-touché: The role of self-touch in the representation of body structure. Consciousness and Cognition,18, 2–11.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Senju, A., Tojo, Y., Dairoku, H., & Hasegawa, T. (2004). Reflexive orientating in response to eye gaze and an arrow in children with and without autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,45(3), 445–458.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shams, L., Kamitani, Y., & Shimojo, S. (2000). What you see is what you hear. Nature,408(6814), 788.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, D. M., & Wallace, M. T. (2016). Dysfunction of sensory oscillations in autism spectrum disorder. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews,68, 848–861.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C., Nicholls, M. E. R., & Driver, J. (2001). The cost of expecting events in the wrong sensory modality. Perception & Psychophysics,63, 330–336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein, B. E., & Meredith, M. A. (1993). The merging of the senses. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein, B. E., Stanford, T. R., & Rowland, B. A. (2014). Development of multisensory integration from the perspective of the individual neuron. Nature Reviews Neuroscience,15(8), 520–535.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, R. A., Segers, M., Ferber, S., Barense, M. D., Camarata, S., & Wallace, M. T. (2016). Keeping time in the brain: Autism spectrum disorder and audiovisual temporal processing. Autism Research,9, 720–738.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, R. A., Segers, M., Ncube, B. L., Black, K. R., Bebko, J. M., Ferber, S., et al. (2017). The cascading influence of multisensory processing on speech perception in autism. Autism,22(5), 609–624.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, R. A., Siemann, J. K., Schneider, B. C., Eberly, H. E., Woynaroski, T. G., Camarata, S. M., et al. (2014a). Multisensory temporal integration in autism spectrum disorders. The Journal of Neuroscience,34(3), 691–697.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, R. A., Siemann, J. K., Woynaroski, T. G., Schneider, B. C., Eberly, H. E., Camarata, S. M., et al. (2014b). Brief report: Arrested development of audiovisual speech perception in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,44(6), 1470–1477.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tavassoli, T., Bellesheim, K., Tommerdahl, M., Holden, J. M., Kolevzon, A., & Buxbaum, J. D. (2016). Altered tactile processing in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research,9(6), 616–620.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, N., Isaac, C., & Milne, E. (2010). A comparison of the development of audiovisual integration in children with autism spectrum disorders and typically developing children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder,40(11), 1403–1411.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thye, M. D., Bednarz, H. M., Herringshaw, A. J., Sartin, E. B., & Kana, R. K. (2018). The impact of atypical sensory processing on social impairments in autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience,29, 151–167.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Todd, J. W. (1912). Reaction time to multiple stimuli. Archives of Psychology,3, 1–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomchek, S. D., & Dunn, W. (2007). Sensory processing in children with and without autism: A comparative study using the short sensory profile. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy,61, 190–200.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Townsend, J. T., & Ashby, F. G. (1978). Methods of modeling capacity in simple processing systems. In N. J. Castellan & F. Restle (Eds.), Cognitive theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich, R., Miller, J., & Schröter, H. (2007). Testing the race model inequality: An algorithm and computer programs. Behavior Research Methods,39(2), 291–302.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Atteveldt, N., Murray, M. M., Thut, G., & Schroeder, C. E. (2014). Multisensory integration: Flexible use of general operations. Neuron,81(6), 1240–1253.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wainwright-Sharp, J. W., & Bryson, S. E. (1993). Visual orienting deficits in high functioning people with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,23, 1–13.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wallace, M. T., & Stevenson, R. A. (2014). The construct of the multisensory temporal binding window and its dysregulation in developmental disabilities. Neuropsychologia,64, 105–123.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, D. L., Goldstein, G., & Minshew, N. J. (2013). The modality shift experiment in adults and children with high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,43, 794–806.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Woynaroski, T. G., Kwakye, L. D., Foss-Feig, J. H., Stevenson, R. A., Stone, W. L., & Wallace, M. T. (2013). Multisensory speech perception in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,43, 2891–2902.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Patricia Jelenic for her help with participant recruitment and selection. OC is a research associate at the National Fund for Scientific Research-Belgium.

Funding

This research was supported in part by the Canada Research Chair Program (FL), the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (AB, FL), the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (FL, GC) and the Research Center of the University Hospital Sainte-Justine (OC).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

GC, SG, and OC designed research; GC, AB, MV, LM contributed in recruiting the participants; GC, MV performed the research; OC, MP, AB, LM and FL contributed unpublished reagents/analytic tools; CB and MP analyzed data under the supervision of OC; GC and OC wrote the first draft of the paper; all authors commented and approved the final version of the paper.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Olivier Collignon.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 263 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Charbonneau, G., Bertone, A., Véronneau, M. et al. Within- and Cross-Modal Integration and Attention in the Autism Spectrum. J Autism Dev Disord 50, 87–100 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04221-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04221-8

Keywords

Navigation