Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Associations Between Conceptual Reasoning, Problem Solving, and Adaptive Ability in High-functioning Autism

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Abstract thinking is generally highly correlated with problem-solving ability which is predictive of better adaptive functioning. Measures of conceptual reasoning, an ecologically-valid laboratory measure of problem-solving, and a report measure of adaptive functioning in the natural environment, were administered to children and adults with and without autism. The individuals with autism had weaker conceptual reasoning ability than individuals with typical development of similar age and cognitive ability. For the autism group, their flexible thinking scores were significantly correlated with laboratory measures of strategy formation and rule shifting and with reported overall adaptive behavior but not socialization scores. Therefore, in autism, flexibility of thought is potentially more important for adaptive functioning in the natural environment than conceptual reasoning or problem-solving.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, W. V., & Sheslow, B. V. (1983). A developmental perspective of adolescence. In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), Autism in adolescents and adults (pp. 11–36). New York, NY: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., rev. ed.). Washington, D.C.: Author.

  • Baez, S., Rattazzi, A., Gonzalez-Gadea, M. L., Torralva, T., Vigliecca, N. S., Decety, J., et al. (2012). Integrating intention and context: Assessing social cognition in adults with Asperger syndrome. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(302), 1–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B (Methodological), 57, 289–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bogte, H., Flamma, B., van der Meere, J., & van Engeland, H. (2007). Cognitive flexibility in adults with high functioning autism. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 11, 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boltë, S., & Poustka, F. (2002). The relation between general cognitive level and adaptive behavior domains in individuals with autism with and without co-morbid mental retardation. Child Psychiatric and Human Development, 33, 165–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chamberlain, E. (2003). Behavioural assessment of the dysexecutive syndrome (BADS): Test review. Journal of Occupational Psychology, Employment and Disability, 5, 33–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillside, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farley, M. A., McMahon, W. M., Fombonne, E., Jenson, W. R., Miller, J., Gardner, M., et al. (2009). Twenty-year outcome for individuals with autism and average or near-average cognitive abilities. Autism Research, 2, 109–118.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fenton, G., D’Ardia, C., Valente, D., Del Vecchio, I., Fabrizi, A., & Bernabei, P. (2001). Vineland adaptive behavior profiles in children with autism and moderate to severe developmental delay. Autism, 7, 269–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, G. (1996). Functional considerations in neuropsychology. In R. J. Shordone & C. J. Long (Eds.), Ecological validity of neuropsychological testing (pp. 75–89). Delray Beach, FL: Gr Press/St Lucie Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, G., Johnson, C. R., & Minshew, N. J. (2001). Attentional processes in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 433–440.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halstead, W. C., & Settlage, P. H. (1943). Grouping behavior of normal persons and persons with lesions of the brain. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 49, 489–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayashi, M., Kato, M., Igarashi, K., & Kashima, H. (2008). Superior fluid intelligence in children with Asperger’s disorder. Brain and Cognition, 66(3), 306–310.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heaton, R. K., Chelune, C. J., Talley, J. L., Kay, G. G., & Curtiss, G. (1993). Wisconsin card sorting test manual, revised and expanded. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heaton, R. K., & Drexler, M. (1987). Clinical and neuropsychological findings in schizophrenia and aging. In N. E. Miller & G. D. Cohen (Eds.), Schizophrenia and aging (pp. 145–161). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, E. L., & Bird, C. M. (2006). Executive processes in Asperger syndrome: Patterns of performance in a multiple case series. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2822–2835.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hollingshead, A. B. (1957). Two-factor index of social position. New Haven, CT: Yale University, Department of Sociology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hooper, H. E. (1983). Hooper visual organization test (VOT). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kandalaft, M. R., Didehbani, N., Krawczyk, D. C., Allen, T. T., & Chapman, S. B. (2013). Virtual reality social cognition training for young adults with high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(1), 34–44.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kanne, S. M., Gerber, A. J., Quirmbach, L. M., Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Saulnier, C. A. (2011). The role of adaptive behavior in autism spectrum disorders: Implications for functional outcome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 1007–1018.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Saulnier, C. A., Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., Volkmar, F. R., & Lord, C. (2007). Social and communication abilities and disabilities in higher-functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders: The Vineland and ADOS. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 748–759.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kourkoulou, A., Leekam, S. R., & Findlay, J. M. (2012). Implicit learning of local context in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(2), 244–256.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krasny, L., Williams, B. J., Provencal, S., & Ozonoff, S. (2003). Social skills interventions for the autism spectrum: Essential ingredients and a model curriculum. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics in North America, 12(1), 107–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laine, M., & Butters, N. (1982). A preliminary study of the problem solving strategies of detoxified long-tern alcoholics. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 10, 235–242.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LeCouteur, A., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Rios, P., Robertson, S., Holdgrafer, M., et al. (1989). Autism diagnostic interview: A standardized investigator-based instrument. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 363–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liss, M., Harel, B., Fein, D., Allen, D., Dunn, M., Feinstein, C., et al. (2001). Predictors and correlates of adaptive functioning in children with developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 219–230.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H, Jr, Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule—generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Goode, S. (1989). Autism diagnostic observation schedule: A standardized investigator-based instrument. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 185–212.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & LeCouteur, A. L. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacAfee, J. (2002). Navigating the social world: A curriculum for individuals with Asperger’s syndrome, high functioning autism and related disorders. Arlington, TX: Future Horizons Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLean, J. E., Szatmari, P., Jones, M. B., Bryson, S. E., Mahoney, W. J., Bartolucci, G., et al. (1999). Familial factors influence level of functioning in pervasive developmental disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 746–753.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mazefsky, C. A., Williams, D. L., & Minshew, N. J. (2008). Variability in adaptive behavior in Autism: Evidence for the importance of family history. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 591–599.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G. (2001). The pattern of intact and impaired memory functions in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 1095–1101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., & Siegel, D. J. (1995). Speech and language in high-functioning autistic individuals. Neuropsychology, 9, 255–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., & Siegel, D. J. (1997). Neuropsychologic functioning in autism: Profile of a complex information processing disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 303–316.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N., Luna, B., & Sweeney, J. (1999). Oculomotor evidence for neocortical systems but not cerebellar dysfunctions in autism. Neurology, 52, 917–922.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N. J., Meyer, J., & Goldstein, G. (2002). Abstract reasoning in autism: A dissociation between concept formation and concept identification. Neuropsychology, 16, 327–334.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N. J., Sung, K., Jones, B., & Furman, J. (2004). Underdevelopment of the postural control system in autism. Neurology, 63, 2056–2061.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newsom, C., & Hovanitz, C. A. (1997). Autistic disorder. In E. J. Mash & L. G. Terdal (Eds.), Assessment of childhood disorders (3rd ed., pp. 408–452). New York, NY: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oswald, D. P., & DiSalvo, C. A. (2003). Adaptive behavior assessment. In T. H. Ollendick & C. S. Schroeder (Eds.), The encyclopedia of pediatric and clinical child psychology. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmen, A., Didden, R., & Lang, R. (2012). A systematic review of behavioral intervention research on adaptive skill building in high-functioning young adults with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 602–617.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 51–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peppé, S., McCann, J., Gibbon, F., O’Hare, A., & Rutherford, M. (2007). Receptive and expressive prosodic ability in children with high-functioning autism. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 50, 1015–1028.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rao, P. A., Beidel, D. C., & Murray, M. J. (2008). Social skills interventions for children with Asperger’s syndrome or high-functioning autism: A review and recommendations. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(2), 353–361.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1993). The Halstead-retain neuropsychological test battery: Theory and clinical interpretation (2nd ed.). Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M. (1983). Cognitive deficits in the pathogenesis of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 24, 513–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. (1975). The functional equivalence of problem solving skills. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 268–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, M., Buaminger, N., & Rogers, S. J. (2011). Abstract reasoning and friendship in high functioning preadolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(1), 32–43.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Soulières, I., Dawson, M., Gernsbacher, M. A., & Mottron, L. (2011). The level and nature of autistic intelligence II: What about Asperger syndrome? PLoS ONE, 6, e25372.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984). Vineland adaptive behavior scales. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, R. L., Hagen, E. P., & Sattler, J. M. (1986). The stanford-binet intelligence scale (4th ed.). Chicago: Riverside Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, D. L., Goldstein, G., & Minshew, N. J. (2005). Impaired memory for faces and social scenes in autism: Clinical implications of the memory disorder. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, 1–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, D. L., Goldstein, G., & Minshew, N. J. (2006a). Neuropsychologic functioning in children with autism: Further evidence of disordered complex information processing. Child Neuropsychology, 12, 279–298.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, D. L., Goldstein, G., & Minshew, N. J. (2006b). Profile of memory function in children with autism. Neuropsychology, 20, 21–29.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, B. A., Alderman, N., Burgess, P. W., Emslie, H., & Evans, J. J. (1996). Behavioural assessment dysexecutive syndrome. Bury St. Edmunds, Suffolk, England: Thames Valley Test Company.

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by Grant Sponsor: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD), Grant Number: HD35469, a Collaborative Program of Excellence in Autism (CPEA); Grant Sponsor: National Institute on Deafness and other Communication Disorders (NIDCD), Grant Number K23DC006691 to Dr. Williams and by the VISN IV Mental Illness Research, Education and Clinical Center (MIRECC), VA Pittsburgh Healthcare System Pittsburgh PA.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gerald Goldstein.

Additional information

The research described here has been approved by the appropriate IRB and full informed consent or assent has been obtained for all subjects.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Williams, D.L., Mazefsky, C.A., Walker, J.D. et al. Associations Between Conceptual Reasoning, Problem Solving, and Adaptive Ability in High-functioning Autism. J Autism Dev Disord 44, 2908–2920 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2190-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2190-y

Keywords

Navigation