Skip to main content
Log in

Evidence for Vocal Flexibility in Wild Siamang (Symphalangus syndactylus) Ululating Scream Phrases

  • Published:
International Journal of Primatology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Nonhuman primate vocalizations have been traditionally described as stereotyped and most likely genetically determined. However, there is increasing evidence of flexibility with a wide variety of species demonstrating the ability to change aspects of their calls, such as note order and phrase duration. We assess patterns of variation in note order and temporal features in wild siamang (Symphalangus syndactylus) phrases. We used three types of analyses on 1,015 ululating scream phrases (US-II), containing 21,609 notes, produced by ten siamang groups at three field sites. First, to assess similarity or differences in the organization of notes in the US-II phrase, we calculated the Levenshtein distance (LD), which quantifies the similarity of sequences of strings through the number of insertions, deletions, and substitutions required to transform one into the other. Second, we used a supervised classification approach to see how well we could assign US-II phrases to their groups and sites. Third, we compared five unsupervised clustering algorithms to investigate the tendency to cluster in US-II phrase types. The note order of the US-II phrase was variable shown by relatively large LD values; there also were significant mean differences between groups. Supervised classification using support vector machine and leave-one-out cross-validation returned 89.5% accuracy for site and 53.1% for group. For unsupervised clustering, the most stable solution based on the Silhouette coefficient returned four unique clusters or classes of the US-II phrase in our dataset. Our findings indicate high levels of intragroup variation in the US-II phrase and are consistent with previous reports on siamang vocalizations being flexible. Quantifying variation in the siamang US-II phrase provides a crucial first step in understanding the evolutionary forces that shaped these signals and the potential information they convey.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Angulo-Chavira, A. Q., & Arias-Trejo, N. (2021). Mediated semantic priming interference in toddlers as seen through pupil dynamics. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 208, 105146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baker-Medard, M., Baker, M. C., & Logue, D. (2013). Chorus song of the indri (Indri indri: Primate, Lemuridae): Group differences and analysis of within-group vocal interactions. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 26(4), 241–255. Retrieved from https://escholarship.org/uc/item/0gg070fd

  • Barreda, S. (2015). phonTools: Functions for phonetics in R. R package version 0.2–2.1.

  • Baxter, P., Bekkering, H., Dijkstra, T., Droop, M., Van den Hurk, M., & Leoné, F. (2022). Contrasting orthographically similar words facilitates adult second language vocabulary learning. Learning and Instruction, 80, 101582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boncoraglio, G., & Saino, N. (2007). Habitat structure and the evolution of bird song: a meta-analysis of the evidence for the acoustic adaptation hypothesis. Functional Ecology, (21), 134–142.

  • Brock, G., Pihur, V., Datta, S., & Datta, S. (2008). clValid: An R package for cluster validation. Journal of Statistical Software, 25, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brockelman, W., & Schilling, D. (1984). Inheritance of stereotyped gibbon calls. Nature, 312(5995), 634–636.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brockelman, W. (1984). Social behaviour of gibbons: Introduction. In H. Preuschoft, D. J. Chivers, W. Y. Brockelman, & N. Creel (Eds.), The lesser apes: Evolutionary and behavioural biology (pp. 285–290). Edinburgh University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brockelman, W., & Srikosamatara, S. (1984). Maintenance and evolution of social structure in gibbons. In H. Preuschoft, D. J. Chivers, W. Y. Brockelman, & N. Creel (Eds.), The lesser apes: Evolutionary and behavioural biology (pp. 298–323). Edinburgh University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brockelman, W., & Srikosamatara, S. (1993). Estimation of density of gibbon groups by use of loud songs. American Journal of Primatology, 29(2), 93–108.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cheney, D., & Seyfarth, R. M. (2018). Flexible usage and social function in primate vocalizations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(9), 1974–1979.

  • Chiara, D. G., Anna, Z., Filippo, C., Teresa, R., Daria, V., Valeria, T., & Cristina, G. (2022). Parent-offspring turn-taking dynamics influence parents’ song structure and elaboration in a singing primate. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, (10), 1–14.

  • Chivers, D. (1976). Communication within and between family groups of siamang (Symphalangus syndactylus). Behaviour, 57(1), 116–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Choi, J. Y., Takahashi, D. Y., & Ghazanfar, A. A. (2015). Cooperative vocal control in marmoset monkeys via vocal feedback. Journal of Neurophysiology, 114(1), 274–283.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, E., Reichard, U., & Zuberbühler, K. (2015). Context-specific close-range “hoo” calls in wild gibbons (Hylobates lar). BMC Evolutionary Biology, 15(1), 56.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, E., Reichard, U., & Zuberbühler, K. (2006). The syntax and meaning of wild gibbon songs. PLoS ONE, 1(1), e73.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Clink, D., Tasirin, J., & Klinck, H. (2020a). Vocal individuality and rhythm in male and female duet contributions of a nonhuman primate Gursky’s spectral tarsier. Current Zoology, 66(2), 173–186.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clink, D., Ahmad, A., & Klinck, H. (2020b). Brevity is not a universal in animal communication: Evidence for compression depends on the unit of analysis in small ape vocalizations. Royal Society Open Science, 7(4), 200151.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Clink, D., Zafar, M., Ahmad, A., & Lau, A. (2021). Limited Evidence for Individual Signatures or Site-Level Patterns of Variation in Male Northern Gray Gibbon (Hylobates funereus) Duet Codas. International Journal of Primatology, 42(6), 896–914.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clink, D., Bernard, H., Crofoot, M., & Marshall, A. (2017). Investigating individual vocal signatures and small-scale patterns of geographic variation in female Bornean gibbon (Hylobates muelleri) great calls. International Journal of Primatology, 38(4), 656–671.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clink, D., Charif, R., Crofoot, M., & Marshall, A. (2018a). Evidence for vocal performance constraints in a female nonhuman primate. Animal Behaviour, 141, 85–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clink, D., Grote, M., Crofoot, M., & Marshall, A. (2018b). Understanding sources of variance and correlation among features of Bornean gibbon (Hylobates muelleri) female calls. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 144(2), 698–708.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clink, D., Tasirin, J., & Klinck, H. (2019). Vocal individuality and rhythm in male and female duet contributions of a nonhuman primate. Current Zoology. 66(2), 173–186.

  • Courter, J., & Ritchison, G. (2010). Alarm calls of tufted titmice convey information about predator size and threat. Behavioral Ecology, 21(5), 936–942.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cowlishaw, G. (1992). Song function in gibbons. Behaviour, 121(1), 131–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cowlishaw, G. (1996). Sexual selection and information content in gibbon song bouts. Ethology, 102(2), 272–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dallmann, R., & Geissmann, T. (2001a). Different levels of variability in the female song of wild silvery gibbons (Hylobates moloch). Behaviour, 138(5), 629–648.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dallmann, R., & Geissmann, T. (2001b). Individuality in the female songs of wild silvery gibbons (Hylobates moloch) on Java, Indonesia. Contributions Zoology, 70, 41–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dallmann, R., & Geissmann, T. (2009). Individual and geographical variability in the songs of wild silvery gibbons (Hylobates moloch) on Java, Indonesia. In S. Lappan & D. Whittacker (Eds.), The gibbons: New perspectives on small ape socioecology and population biology (pp. 91–110). Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species by means of natural selection, or, the preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life. J. Murray.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Delarue, J., Todd, S. K., Van Parijs, S. M., & Di Iorio, L. (2009). Geographic variation in Northwest Atlantic fin whale (Balaenoptera physalus) song: Implications for stock structure assessment. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 125(3), 1774–1782.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Egnor, S., & Hauser, M. (2004). A paradox in the evolution of primate vocal learning. Trends in Neuroscience, 27, 649–654.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Engesser, S., & Townsend, S. (2019). Combinatoriality in the vocal systems of nonhuman animals. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 10(4), e1493.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A. (1995). As biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture, (1), 391.

  • Farabaugh, S. (1982). The ecological and social significance of duetting. In D. E. Kroodsma, E. H. Miller, & H. Ouellet (Eds.), Acoustic communication in birds (pp. 85–124). Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, J., Hammerschmidt, K., Cheney, D., & Seyfarth, R. (2002). Acoustic features of male baboon loud calls: Influences of context, age, and individuality. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 111(3), 1465–1474.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Freeberg, T., Dunbar, R., & Ord, T. (2012). Social complexity as a proximate and ultimate factor in communicative complexity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 367, 1785–1801.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frey, B., & Dueck, D. (2007). Clustering by passing messages between data points. Science, 315(5814), 972–976.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gamba, M., Torti, V., Bonadonna, G., Randrianarison, R. M., Friard, O., & Giacoma, C. (2016a). Melody in my head, melody in my genes? Acoustic similarity, individuality, and genetic relatedness in the indris of Eastern Madagascar. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 140(4), 3017–3018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gamba, M., Torti, V., Estienne, V., Randrianarison, R. M., Valente, D., Rovara, P., & Giacoma, C. (2016b). The indris have got rhythm! Timing and pitch variation of a primate song examined between sexes and age classes. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 10, 249.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gamba, M., Colombo, C., & Giacoma, C. (2012). Acoustic cues to caller identity in lemurs: A case study. Journal of Ethology, 30(1), 191–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garland, E., Rendell, L., Lamoni, L., Poole, M., & Noad, M. (2017). Song hybridization events during revolutionary song change provide insights into cultural transmission in humpback whales. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(30), 7822–7829.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Garland, E., Lilley, M., Goldizen, A., Rekdahl, M., Garrigue, C., & Noad, M. (2012). Improved versions of the Levenshtein distance method for comparing sequence information in animals’ vocalizations: Tests using humpback whale song. Behaviour, 149, 1413–1441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garland, E., Noad, M., Goldizen, A., Lilley, M., Rekdahl, M., Garrigue, C., & Robbins, J. (2013). Quantifying humpback whale song sequences to understand the dynamics of song exchange at the ocean basin scale. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 133(1), 560–569.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geissmann, T. (2002). Duet-splitting and the evolution of gibbon songs. Biology Reviews, 77(1), 57–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geissmann, T. (1984). Inheritance of song parameters in the gibbon song, analysed in 2 hybrid gibbons (Hylobates pileatus and H lar). Folia Primatologica., 42, 216–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geissmann, T. (1986). Mate change enhances duetting activity in the siamang gibbon (Hylobates syndactulus). Behaviour, 96(1), 17–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geissmann, T., & Orgeldinger, M. (2000). The relationship between duet songs and pair bonds in siamangs (Hylobates syndactylus). Animal Behaviour, 60, 805–809.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geissmann, T. (2000). Duet songs of the siamang (Hylobates syndactylus) I. Structure and organization. Primate Report, (1), 33–60.

  • Geissmann, T. (1999). Duet songs of the siamang (Hylobates syndactylus) II. Testing the pair-bonding hypothesis during a partner exchange. Behaviour136(8), 1005–1039.

  • Gil, D., & Slater, P. (2000). Song organization and singing patterns of the willow warbler, (Phylloscopus trochilus). Behaviour, 137(6), 759–782.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gooskens, C., & Heeringa, W. (2004). Perceptive evaluation of Levenshtein dialect distance measurements using Norwegian dialect data. Language Variation and Change, 16(3), 189–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Güttinger, H. (1979). The Integration of Learnt and Genetically Programmed Behaviour: A Study of Hierarchical Organization in Songs of Canaries, Greenfinches and their Hybrids 1. Zeitschrift Für Tierpsychologie, 49(3), 285–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haimoff, E. (1981). Video analysis of siamang (Hylobates syndactylus) songs. Behaviour, 76(1), 128–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haimoff, E. (1984). Acoustic and organizational features of gibbon songs. The Lesser Apes. Evolutionary and behavioural biology. Edinburgh University Press (1984), (pp. 333–353).

  • Haimoff, E. (1988). The organization of repair in the songs of gibbons. Semiotica, 68, 89–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haimoff, E. (1986). Convergence in the duetting of monogamous old-world primates. Journal of Human Evolution, 15, 51–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammerschmidt, K., & Fischer, J. (2008). Constraints in primate vocal production. In U. Griebel & K. Oller (Eds.), The evolution of communicative creativity: From fixed signals to contextual flexibility (pp. 93–119). The MIT Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Haraway, M., & Maples, E. (1998). Flexibility in the species-typical songs of gibbons. Primates, 39, 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. (1991). Sources of Acoustic Variation in Rhesus Macaque (Macaca mulatta) Vocalizations. Ethology, 89(1), 29–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helweg, D., Cato, D., Jenkins, P., Garrigue, C., & McCauley, R. (1998). Geographic variation in South Pacific humpback whale songs. Behaviour, 135, 1–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hocket, C. (1960). The origin of speech. Scientific American, 603, 3–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurford, J., & Hurford, J. (2012). The origins of grammar: Language in the light of evolution II (Vol. 2). Oxford University Press.

  • Inoue, Y., Sinun, W., & Okanoya, K. (2021). Increase in social interactions of wild Northern Gray gibbons (Hylobates funereus) during the mast fruiting period in the Danum Valley Conservation Area, Sabah, Malaysia. Acta Ethologica, 24(3), 153–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, A., Ten Cate, C., & Bijleveld, C. (2001). The interobserver reliability of scoring sonagrams by eye: A study on methods, illustrated on zebra finch songs. Animal Behaviour, 62, 791–801.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kershenbaum, A., Bowles, A., Freeberg, T., Jin, D., Lameira, A., & Bohn, K. (2014). Animal vocal sequences: Not the Markov chains we thought they were. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1792), 20141370.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kershenbaum, A., Blumstein, D., Roch, M., Akçay, Ç., Backus, G., Bee, M., & Coen, M. (2016). Acoustic sequences in non-human animals: A tutorial review and prospectus. Biological Reviews, 91(1), 13–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kershenbaum, A., & Garland, E. C. (2015). Quantifying similarity in animal vocal sequences: Which metric performs best? Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 6, 1452–1461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koda, H. (2016). Gibbon songs: Understanding the evolution and development of this unique form of vocal communication. In Evolution of Gibbons and Siamang (pp. 347–357).

  • Koda, H., Nishimura, T., Tokuda, I. T., Oyakawa, C., Nihonmatsu, T., & Masataka, N. (2012). Soprano singing in gibbons. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 149(3), 347–355. Springer.

  • Koda, H., Lemasson, A., Oyakawa, C., Pamungkas, J., & Masataka, N. (2013). Possible role of mother-daughter vocal interactions on the development of species-specific song in gibbons. PLoS ONE, 8(8), e71432.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kohonen, T. (1985). Median strings. Pattern Recognition Letters, 3, 309–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohonen, T. (1997). Self-organizing maps. Springer-Verlag, second edition.

  • Konopka. T. (2022). _umap: Uniform manifold approximation and projection_. R package version 0.2.8.0, <https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=umap>

  • Ku-Peralta, W., Navarro-Sigüenza, A., Sandoval, L., & Sosa-López, J. (2020). Geographic variation in the duets of the Rufous-naped Wren (Campylorhynchus rufinucha) complex. The Auk137(3), ukaa015.

  • Lameira, A., & Wich, S. (2008). Orangutan long call degradation and individuality over distance: A playback approach. International Journal of Primatology, 29(3), 615–625.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamprecht, J. (1970). Duettgesang beim Siamang, Symphalangus syndactylus (Hominoidea, Hylobatinae). Zeitschrift Für Tierpsychologie, 27(2), 186–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lau, A., Clink, D., & Bales, K. (2020). Individuality in the vocalizations of infant and adult coppery titi monkeys (Plecturocebus cupreus). American Journal of Primatology, 82(6), e23134.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Leliveld, L. M., Scheumann, M., & Zimmermann, E. (2011). Acoustic correlates of individuality in the vocal repertoire of a nocturnal primate (Microcebus murinus). The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 129(4), 2278–2288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Macedonia, J. (1986). Individuality in a contact call of the ringtailed lemur (Lemur catta). American Journal of Primatology, 11(2), 163–179.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Margoliash, D., Staicer, C., & Inoue, S. (1991). Stereotyped and plastic song in adult indigo buntings (Passerina cyanea). Animal Behaviour, 42, 367–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, J., & Marshall, E. (1976). Gibbons and their territorial songs. Science, 193(4249), 235–237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McInnes, L., Healy, J., & Melville, J. (2018). Umap: Uniform manifold approximation and projection for dimension reduction. arXiv preprint arXiv:1802.03426.

  • Merguerditchian, A., Vuillemin, A., & Pruetz, J. (2018). Identifying the Ape Beat in the Wild: Rhythmic Indvidual Signatures from Sounds of Manual Fruit Cracking in Fongoli Chimpanzees. In The Evolution of Language Proceedings of the 12th International Conference on the Evolution of Language (Evolang12) pp. 293–294.

  • Miller, C., Flusberg, S., & Hauser, M. (2003). Interruptibility of long call production in tamarins: Implications for vocal control. Journal of Experimental Biology, 206(15), 2629–2639.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mitani, J., Gros-Louis, J., & Macedonia, J. (1996). Selection for acoustic individuality within the vocal repertoire of wild chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology, 17(4), 569–583.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, B., Harwood, B., & Drummond, T. (2018). Nearest neighbour radial basis function solvers for deep neural networks. https://www.openreview.net/forum?id=SkFEGHx0Z

  • Mitani, J. (1988). Male gibbon (Hylobates agilis) singing behavior: Natural history, song variations and function. Ethology, 79(3), 177–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitani J. (1985). Gibbon Song Duets and Intergroup Spacing. Behaviour, 92: 59–96. Mitani, J., & Rodman, P. (1979). Territoriality: the relation of ranging pattern and home range size to defendability, with an analysis of territoriality among primate species. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology5(3), 241–251.

  • Morton, E. (1975). Ecological sources of selection on avian sounds. The American Naturalist, 109(965), 17–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mundinger, P. (1980). Animal cultures and a general theory of cultural evolution. Ethology and Sociobiology, 1, 183–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mundry, R., & Sommer, C. (2007). Discriminant function analysis with nonindependent data: Consequences and an alternative. Animal Behaviour, 74(4), 965–976.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nottebohm, F. (1970). Ontogeny of Bird Song: Different strategies in vocal development are reflected in learning stages, critical periods, and neural lateralization. Science, 167(3920), 950–956.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Podos, J., & Warren, P. S. (2007). The evolution of geographic variation in birdsong. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 37, 403–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rauber, R., Kranstauber, B., & Manser, M. (2020). Call order within vocal sequences of meerkats contains temporary contextual and individual information. BMC Biology, 18(1), 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Řeháková-Petrů, M., Policht, R., and Peške, L. (2012). Acoustic repertoire of the Philippine tarsier (Tarsius syrichta fraterculus) and individual variation of long-distance calls. International Journal of Zoology, (4), 1–10.

  • Rousseeuw, P. (1987). Silhouettes: A graphical aid to the interpretation and validation of cluster analysis. Journal of Computational and Applied Mathematics, 20, 53–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rukstalis, M., Fite, J., & French, J. (2003). Social change affects vocal structure in a callitrichid primate (Callithrix kuhlii). Ethology, 109(4), 327–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salmi, R., Hammerschmidt, K., & Doran-Sheehy, D. (2014). Individual distinctiveness in call types of wild western female gorillas. PLoS ONE, 9(7), e101940.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sainburg, T., Thielk, M., & Gentner, T. Q. (2020). Finding, visualizing, and quantifying latent structure across diverse animal vocal repertoires. PLoS Computational Biology, 16(10), e1008228.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Scrucca, L., Fop, M., Murphy, T., & Raftery, A. (2016). mclust 5: Clustering, classification and density estimation using Gaussian finite mixture models. The R Journal, 8(1), 289.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Seddon, N. (2005). Ecological adaptation and species recognition drives vocal evolution in neotropical suboscine birds. Evolution, 59(1), 200–215.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth, R., & Cheney, D. (1997). Some general features of vocal development in nonhuman primates. In C. T. Snowdon & M. Hausberger (Eds.), Social influences on vocal development (pp. 249–273). Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth, R., & Cheney, D. (2010). Production, usage, and comprehension in animal vocalizations. Brain Language, 115(1), 92–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth, R., & Cheney, D. (2018). Pragmatic flexibility in primate vocal production. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 21, 56–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singletary, B., & Tecot, S. (2020). Multimodal pair-bond maintenance: A review of signaling across modalities in pair-bonded nonhuman primates. American Journal of Primatology, 82(3), e23105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snowdon, C. (2009). Plasticity of communication in nonhuman primates. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 40, 239–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowdon, C. (2017). Vocal communication in family-living and pair-bonded primates. In Primate Hearing and Communication. (63), 141–174.

  • Snowdon, C., & de la Torre, S. (2002). Multiple environmental contexts and communication in pygmy marmosets (Cebuella pygmaea). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 116(2), 182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snowdon, C., & Boe, C. (2003). Social communication about unpalatable foods in tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117(2), 142.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sun, K., Luo, L., Kimball, R. T., Wei, X., Jin, L., Jiang, T., & Feng, J. (2013). Geographic variation in the acoustic traits of greater horseshoe bats: Testing the importance of drift and ecological selection in evolutionary processes. PLoS ONE, 8(8), e70368.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki, T., Wheatcroft, D., & Griesser, M. (2016). Experimental evidence for compositional syntax in bird calls. Nature Communications, 7(1), 1–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Symmes, D., Newman, J., Talmage-Riggs, G., & Lieblich, A. (1979). Individuality and stability of isolation peeps in squirrel monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 27, 1142–1152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tanaka, T., Sugiura, H., & Masataka, N. (2006a). Sound transmission in the habitats of Japanese macaques and its possible effect on population differences in coo calls. Behaviour, 143(8), 993–1012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tanaka, T., Sugiura, H., & Masataka, N. (2006b). Cross-sectional and longitudinal studies of the development of group differences in acoustic features of coo calls in two groups of Japanese macaques. Ethology, 112(1), 7–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Templeton, C., Greene, E., & Davis, K. (2005). Allometry of alarm calls: Black-capped chickadees encode information about predator size. Science, 308(5730), 1934–1937.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tenaza, R. (1985). Songs of hybrid gibbons (Hylobates lar and H. muelleri). American Journal of Primatology, 8(3), 249–253.

  • Terleph, T., Malaivijitnond, S., & Reichard, U. (2018a). Male white-handed gibbons flexibly time duet contributions. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 72(1), 16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terleph, T., Malaivijitnond, S., & Reichard, U. (2018b). An analysis of white-handed gibbon male song reveals speech-like phrases. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 166(3), 649–660.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Terleph, T., Malaivijitnond, S., & Reichard, U. (2016). Age related decline in female lar gibbon great call performance suggests that call features correlate with physical condition. BMC Evolutionary Biology, 16(1), 4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Terleph, T., Malaivijitnond, S., & Reichard, U. (2015). Lar gibbon (Hylobates lar) great call reveals individual caller identity. American Journal of Primatology, 77(7), 811–821.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tougaard, J., & Eriksen, N. (2006). Analysing differences among animal songs quantitatively by means of the Levenshtein distance measure. Behaviour, 143, 239–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Loo, M. (2014). The stringdist package for approximate string matching. R J., 6(1), 111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, B. C., Fahy, M., & Tiddi, B. (2019). Experimental evidence for heterospecific alarm signal recognition via associative learning in wild capuchin monkeys. Animal Cognition, 22(5), 687–695.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wieling, M., Montemagni, S., Nerbonne, J., & Baayen, R. (2014). Lexical differences between Tuscan dialects and standard Italian: Accounting for geographic and sociodemographic variation using generalized additive mixed modeling. Language, 90, 669–692.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkins, M., Seddon, N., & Safran, R. (2012). Evolutionary divergence in acoustic signals: Causes and consequences. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 28, 156–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yuan, Z., Zhou, W., Chen, X., Poyarkov, N., Jr., Chen, H., Jang-Liaw, N. H., & Che, J. (2016). Spatiotemporal diversification of the true frogs (genus Rana): A historical framework for a widely studied group of model organisms. Systematic Biology, 65(5), 824–842.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yurk, H., Barrett-Lennard, L., Ford, J., & Matkin, C. (2002). Cultural transmission within maternal lineages: Vocal clans in resident killer whales in southern Alaska. Animal Behaviour, 63(6), 1103–1119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zanoli, A., De Gregorio, C., Valente, D., Torti, V., Bonadonna, G., Randrianarison, R., & Gamba, M. (2020). Sexually dimorphic phrase organization in the song of the indris (Indri indri). American Journal of Primatology, 82(6), e23132.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler, K. (2005). The phylogenetic roots of language: Evidence from primate communication and cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(3), 126–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler, K., & Lemasson, A. (2014). Primate communication: Meaning from strings of calls In Language and recursion (pp. 115–125). Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler, K. (2000). Referential labelling in Diana monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 59(5), 917–927.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbuhler, K. (2003). Referential signaling in non-human primates: Cognitive precursors and limitations for the evolution of language. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 33, 265–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This project was funded through the Fulbright U.S. Student Research Program with a 2017-2018 grant. Further funding was provided by the SIU Graduate and Professional Student Council (GPSC) Scholarship. This project followed the permits and protocol approvals of the Illinois Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) at Southern Illinois University (protocol number: 17-007) and the foreign research approval from the Indonesia Ministry of Research and Technology (RISTEK-315-IM2KAB20559). This project also collaborated with the conservation organizations Yayasan Ekosistem Lestari (YEL), the Sumatran Orangutan Conservation Programme (SOCP), and Taman Nasional Gunung Leuser (TNGL). The authors thank the American Indonesian Exchange Foundation (AMINEF), key supporters Rudi Putra, Matthew Nowak, Isa, Rizqi, and field assistants Supri, Asan, Aulia, Sahril, Dian, Ucok, Wandi, Yansa, Alwi, Fredri, Abdusalam, Roni, Ziva Justinek, and Amelie Desmoulin. They also thank Dr. Ulrich Reichard and Dr. Thomas Terleph for their guidance and support as well as Dr. Ellen Garland, Dr. Marco Gamba, and Anna Zanoli for their help with the Levenshtein distance data analysis. Thanks to the two reviewers and the editor who provided comments and questions that improved the final version of this paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JD and DC conceived the research design and methodology. JD conducted fieldwork with the guidance and support of AA. JD annotated the spectrogram data. JD performed the Levenshtein Distance (LD) and ANOVA statistical analyses. DC assisted with LD analyses and performed the classification statistical analyses. JD and DC wrote the manuscript; SS and AA provided guidance and editorial advice.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Justin D’Agostino.

Ethics declarations

Inclusion and Diversity Statement

Statements from the IJP instructions for authors (2022) that relate to our manuscript:

  • One or more of the authors of this paper self-identifies as an underrepresented ethnic minority in science.

  • The author list includes contributors from the location where the research was conducted, who participated in study conception, study design, data collection, analysis, and/or interpretation of the findings.

Additional information

Handling Editor: Joanna (Jo) M. Setchell

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 6331 KB)

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

D’Agostino, J., Spehar, S., Abdullah, A. et al. Evidence for Vocal Flexibility in Wild Siamang (Symphalangus syndactylus) Ululating Scream Phrases. Int J Primatol 44, 1127–1148 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-023-00384-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-023-00384-5

Keywords

Navigation