Statistical inference is at the heart of statistics, as it provides a means to make substantive evidence-based claims under uncertainty when only partial data are available (Makar & Rubin, 2018, p. 262). Interpreting inferences with associated uncertainty is difficult for students, which is why, in most countries, inferences are not taught until Grade 10 or higher. Students’ difficulties in learning inferences mostly relate to limited understanding of key statistical concepts, such as sample, variability, and distributions, and to problems with understanding complex formal procedures (Castro Sotos et al., 2007). Engaging in activities that involve informal inferences in the early years, within primary education or early years of secondary school, seems to facilitate learning about more complex inferential statistics later on (Makar & Rubin, 2009; Van Dijke-Droogers et al., 2020).

However, the pre-grade-10 statistics curriculum in most countries, including the Netherlands, focuses on descriptive statistics without paying attention to inferences—with the exception of for example New Zealand, where a full learning line including inferential activities was developed starting from primary school.Footnote 1 Promising results for informal inferential activities encourage investigating how these can be embedded in current curricula with a descriptive focus. Within most mathematical curricula, only limited time is available for statistics. As such, we need efficient learning trajectories and knowledge about crucial steps in such a trajectory.

In this study, we use knowledge from literature on (informal) statistical inference and apply knowledge on learning progressions to design and evaluate an innovative learning trajectory (LT) on introducing 9th-grade students in the pre-university stream—the 15% best-performing students of the Dutch educational system—to the key concepts for statistical inference. Following Duschl et al. (2011), we will address the following aspects: how the design process included the selection of the core idea of the LT, how theories on statistical inference inform the design of the LT, the identification of the starting and end point of the LT, how the successive learning steps of the LT mediate learning, and how the LT aligns with current curricula. To empirically verify the effects of the designed LT, we implemented the LT at five different Dutch schools with eleven participating teachers and a total of 267 students. We analyzed both students’ performance after the intervention, and their progress during the learning process.

Theoretical Background

Statistical Inference

Statistical inference concerns interpreting sample results, drawing data-based conclusions, and reasoning about probability. For students, it is difficult to understand formal procedures to substantiate their inferences. Many difficulties involve a poor understanding of the key statistical concepts: sample, variability, and distributions. These key concepts, including the understanding of the effect of sample size and the idea that a sample characteristic—such as mean or median—can be used to compare distributions, are essential for understanding inferences (Bakker, 2004; Chance et al., 2004; Konold & Pollatsek, 2002; Saldanha & Thompson, 2002; Watson & Kelly, 2008). There is a strong relationship between these concepts: understanding the sampling distribution relies on understanding the key concept of a sample, in particular on understanding the balance between sample representativeness and sample variability (Batanero et al., 1994). Common misconceptions involve neglecting the effect of sample size on the variance of sample mean or sample proportion (Tversky & Kahneman, 1971). Another common difficulty involves probabilistic reasoning, as students tend to provide deterministic explanations and not to consider the variability involved (Rossman, 2008).

To help students overcome difficulties involved in statistical inference, informal approaches have been sought in recent decades. In general, this informal approach focuses on making inferences about unknown populations based on observed samples without using formal techniques, such as hypothesis testing. Makar and Rubin (2009) define informal statistical inference in main principles: generalization beyond data, data as evidence for these generalizations, and probabilistic reasoning about the generalization. Informal inferences include data-based claims that go beyond the collected data, in which the uncertainty involved can be expressed in informal probabilistic reasoning about the likelihood of the claim. Offering informal activities at an early age—before the more formal activities in Grade 10 or higher—facilitates the understanding of key concepts and probabilistic reasoning required for statistical inference (Paparistodemou & Meletiou-Mavrotheris, 2008; Van Dijke-Droogers et al., 2020).

The Design of a Learning Trajectory

The design of an LT entails a conjectured route through a set of educational activities to support students to achieve the intended learning goals. Although learning is a personal process, unique for each student, a conjectured LT intends to describe a “possible taken-as-shared learning route for the classroom community” (Gravemeijer et al., 2003, p. 52); a learning route needs empirical validation. Successful implementation of theory in educational practice involves the design and evaluation in real classrooms of powerful LTs that embody our present understanding of effective learning (De Corte, 2000).

The theory of Realistic Mathematics Education (Cobb, 2011; Freudenthal, 1983) provides design heuristics for the development of learning activities in an LT. First, the learning activities should be set in a context that enables students to immediately engage and develop associated mathematical concepts. As such, the learning activities support students in progressing toward a toolkit of key concepts associated with the learning goals of the LT. Second, the activities should be structured to support students in developing models of their concrete mathematical activity that can be used as model for a network of mathematical objects and relationships (Gravemeijer, 1999; Streefland, 1991).

The Current Study

The study is part of a larger study to gain knowledge about a theoretically and empirically based learning trajectory to introduce 9th-grade students to the key concepts of statistical inference. From another study (Van Dijke-Droogers et al., submitted) on the overall effects of the LT on students’ statistical literacy, we know that the LT had a significant positive effect on students’ understanding of statistical inference as measured by comparing pre- and posttest results. In the study reported here, we want to know how students learned something about statistical inference in terms of the intended LT-step–related learning goals of the trajectory. When it comes to experimental studies that only report pre-post results, a common concern is that the reader may still not know how to benefit from the intervention reported (Savelsbergh et al., 2016). We therefore consider it worth spelling out in more detail the design of the 8-step LT, and its effects on students’ understanding of LT-step–related goals for statistical inference and analyze students’ progression during the large-scale intervention. As such, we address the following research questions:

  1. 1.

    What are the specific effects of the designed LT on students’ understanding of statistical inference, in terms of the intended LT-step–related learning goals?

  2. 2.

    How do the designed LT steps foster students’ learning processes?

Methods

The designed LT aims at introducing students to key concepts of statistical inference by using theories of informal statistical inference. We first outline the design of the LT. We incorporated two main ideas: repeated sampling with a black box and statistical modeling with a digital tool. Second, we describe the intervention characteristics and data analysis.

An Outline of the LT

This study comprises the results of a third cycle of design-based research. During cycle 1 and 2, the LT was (re)designed, implemented, and evaluated, to identify the feasibility of the LT and to further define the starting and ending points of the LT.

The design of the LT consists of two similar sequences of four learning steps. Sequence I concerns only categorical data and includes the following steps: (1) experimenting with a physical black box, (2) visualizing distributions, (3) examining sampling distributions using simulation software, and (4) interpreting sampling distributions to make inferences in real-life contexts. In Sequence II, following Rossman (2008), more complex numerical data are addressed during LT steps 5 to 8. The first three steps of sequences I and II involved 45 min each. In the last step of sequences I and II, three different real-life contexts were offered with a time duration of 45 min per context. An outline of each LT step including a brief description, examples of learning activities, and the intended learning goals, is presented in Table 1. A more detailed description can be found in the Supplementary Material A.

Table 1 Overview of steps 1–8 of the learning trajectory

Repeated Sampling with a Black Box

Repeated sampling with a black box serves as a guiding activity through all steps of the LT. A black box refers to a box of which only part of the content is visible—for example, a box with a viewing window that is filled with marbles or a box filled with notes (see the pictures in Table 1 at LT Steps 1 and 5, respectively). The black box activities instantiate design heuristics of Realistic Mathematics Education (Cobb, 2011; Freudenthal, 1983). Starting within the engaging context of a physical black box experiment—in both sequences I and II—enables students to immediately involve and orient toward developing key statistical concepts (Van Dijke-Droogers et al., 2020). In Sequence I, activities with a physical black box filled with marbles in LT steps 1 and 2 enable students to explore the sampling variability involved in repeated sampling. Varying the size of the viewing window in the physical black box activities allows students to explore the effects of sample size. These activities incorporate ideas of the growing sample task (Bakker, 2004) and repeated sampling that make key statistical concepts more accessible for students (Van Dijke-Droogers et al., 2020), specifically, when those activities are accompanied by classroom discussions for exchanging and comparing sample results (Wild & Pfannkuch, 1999). The idea of repeated sampling with a physical black box is extended in statistical modeling activities in LT steps 3 and 4. In Sequence II, the activities evolve in a similar way from starting with a physical black box filled with notes in LT steps 5 and 6 to statistical modeling in LT steps 7 and 8.

Statistical Modeling with Digital Technology

Statistical modeling activities with educational digital tools facilitate—on an informal level—the exploration of key concepts for statistical inference (Biehler et al., 2013; Garfield et al., 2015; Manor & Ben-Zvi, 2015; Rossman, 2008; Saldanha & Thompson, 2002; Watson & Chance, 2012). Digital environments such as TinkerPlots provide opportunities to easily simulate and visualize (repeated) samples. In the designed LT, the statistical modeling activities start within the familiar context of a black box, where students build a model of a black box—for example filled with 200 red and 400 blue marbles—to simulate sample results. By visualizing sample and sampling distributions, at varying sample sizes and at varying number of repeated samples, students explore (un)likely sample results. The modeling activities within the black box context gradually evolve to modeling real-life contexts. Modeling activities include building a model, simulating (repeated) samples, visualizing and interpreting the results, to solve a given problem. As with the physical black box activities, these modeling activities attend all stages of the statistical investigation cycle several times, as students collect data, analyze their data using sample and sampling distributions, and interpret the results to answer the question posed. Subsequent modeling activities involve applying gained knowledge into new contexts, where students deploy modeling activities to solve real-life problems.

Applying similar digital techniques within varying contexts encourages students to identify context-independent patterns of technical actions (Van Dijke-Droogers et al., 2021). These context-independent technical patterns combined with a context-independent understanding of key statistical concepts facilitate the conceptual shift from a model of to a model for, known as emergent modeling (Gravemeijer, 1999; Streefland, 1991). As such, statistical modeling enhances the use and understanding of context-independent statistical models, which is essential for interpreting inferences.

Participants

Eleven teachers participated in the intervention, with a total of 267 students (Grade 9, aged 14–15 years) from thirteen classes at five different schools. The teachers were trained for the intervention in two 3-h sessions in which they worked through students’ lessons and materials themselves, guided by the researcher. The teachers decided to replace all regular 9th-grade statistics lessons with the LT to save time. The students had no experience with using digital tools during their mathematics lessons. Students were instructed in using TinkerPlots in LT step 3 through a demonstration by a teacher, and they received an instruction sheet for modeling black boxes that they could use during LT steps 4 to 8. They had some basic knowledge of descriptive statistics: center and distribution measures, such as mean, quartiles, class division, absolute and relative frequencies, and boxplot. A comparison group with students who followed the regular curriculum was used to interpret students’ performance on statistical inference. The comparison group consisted of 217 students from ten classes. All students in the comparison group attended 10–16 regular 9th-grade statistics lessons during their mathematics lessons. The participating students, for both the intervention and comparison group, belonged to the 15% best-performing students in our educational system.

Data Collection and Analysis

For Phase 1, addressing the first research question, we developed a pre- and posttest for statistical inference (SI) at the school level, inspired by Watson and Callingham’s (2003, 2004) work on statistical literacy. Both tests were part of a broader study on the effects of the designed LT on students’ statistical literacy (Van Dijke-Droogers et al., submitted). For the study presented here, we focused on the SI items of the posttest. The posttest contained 18 SI items. We selected four items from Watson and Callingham (2004), and we designed 14 new items related to concepts of SI as addressed in the LT. For the design of the new items, we used the structure and phrasing of their items. To analyze the validity of the designed test, we conducted two pilot tests in different classrooms, each consisting of 25 students. Concerning the concurrent validity of the new designed SI items, students’ average level scores in the pilots on new designed and existing SI items were not significantly different (Mnew = 2.49, SDnew = 0.71, Mex = 2.78, SDex = 1.38, n = 50, t(49) =  − 1.6; p = 0.11). To assess the content and construct validity of test items, the results of each pilot were used for in-depth discussion with experts in this area on content, construct, vocabulary, and clarity. Cronbach’s alpha value was 0.81, indicating a good reliability (Taber, 2018). For the data collection, the participating teachers from both the intervention and comparison group conducted the test, according to a clear instruction for testing, from their own students during their regular 45-min mathematics lessons.

For the data analysis in Phase 1 on the posttest results, we defined six SI levels, based on Watson and Callingham’s levels for statistical literacy (see Table 2). Given that LT steps 1 to 4 and 5 to 8 involve similar concepts and approaches, we defined specific levels for couples of two: steps 1 and 5, steps 2 and 6, steps 3 and 7, steps 4 and 8 (see Supplementary Material B). By pairing the LT steps, we were able to analyze at least four test items per couple. For the coding, we developed item-specific level-codes (e.g. Figs. 2 and 3). Two assessors coded test data from the participating students with the SI level scores 0–6. To indicate students’ performance on the test, we compared students’ test scores for both the intervention and comparison group, and as such, for attending the LT or regular statistics curriculum. Students’ results on the pretest were used to identify students’ initial level. Although the comparison group attended the regular statistics lessons prior to the pretest, we conjectured similar pretest results for both groups on statistical inference as the regular lessons only concerned descriptive statistics. For statistical significance, we used one-way ANOVA for comparing results from both groups, paired t test for analyzing students’ progression between the pre- and posttest, and chi-squared test for comparing students’ distribution over the levels. For reliability of the analysis, a third coder was asked to process independently a random set of 5% (80 items) of the data with students’ reasoning. The third coder agreed on 83% of the codes. Deviating codes, which were limited to one or two levels difference at most, were discussed until agreement was reached. Adjustments in the coding were also applied to the rest of the data.

Table 2 Defined Levels for Statistical Inference based on Levels for Statistical Literacy by Watson and Callingham (2003)

In Phase 2, addressing the second research question, we used two principles by Wilson (2009) for assessing learning progression. The first principle outlines a developmental perspective regarding the development of students’ understanding of particular concepts and skills over time—that is, during the LT instead of assessing final performance. This perspective requires clear definitions and a theoretical framework of what and how students are expected to learn. In our study, these are embedded in the description of the designed 8-step LT. The second principle involves the match between the LT and assessment. To establish a strong match, we formulated indicators for success of each LT step. In the design of the learning activities on students’ worksheets, specific tasks were included that correspond to these indicators. Table 3 displays the indicators and corresponding learning activities on students’ worksheet, for each LT step (see Table 1 for corresponding learning goals in each LT step).

Table 3 Overview of indicators and corresponding learning activities on students’ worksheet, for each LT step

Data included students’ worksheets 1 to 8 from each LT step, accompanied by teachers’ and researchers’ notes. We collected 267 worksheets from Sequence I, LT steps 1 to 4, and 224 worksheets from Sequence II. The teacher took notes about each lesson. After each lesson, the researcher contacted the teacher—through email, call, or a meeting in person—to evaluate the lesson given and to discuss the following steps. In addition, we used researchers’ observation data from two visits in each class about how the teacher and students interacted with the intervention materials. For the data analysis in phase 2, we coded students’ reasoning on their worksheets, for the specific tasks in each LT step, according to the indicators. Students were explicitly asked to clearly motivate their answers on their worksheets. Teachers’ and researchers’ notes were included in the analysis.

Results

We first present students’ results on the posttest to answer research question 1. Next, we present students’ progress during the intervention to address research question 2.

Posttest Results on Students’ Understanding of Statistical Inference

With regard to students’ statistical inference (SI) level at the posttest, we reported in another study (Van Dijke-Droogers et al., submitted) that a one-way ANOVA between both groups indicated that the level score for the intervention group who attended the LT was significantly higher than for the comparison group (+ 0.67, F(1, 482) = 75.0, p < 0.0005). The results in the study presented here indicate that the intervention group scored significantly higher than the comparison group on each coupled LT steps 1 and 5 on using samples, LT steps 2 and 6 on visualizing distributions, LT steps 3 and 7 on repeated sampling and effect of sample size, and LT steps 4 and 8 on solving real-life problems. The results are displayed in Table 4. Although we conjectured a similar pretest score for both groups, the results showed that the initial level of the intervention group on statistical inference was significantly lower than for the comparison group—probably because the comparison group followed their (descriptive) statistics lessons prior to the pretest. The comparison group was not taught statistics between the pre- and posttest, which explains their similar scores on SI at both tests.

Table 4 Students’ mean level scores on the coupled LT steps at the pre- and posttest

We now elaborate on three posttest items for which the results of the intervention and comparison group were quite different. The first item is from Watson and Callingham (2004) and the second and third are newly designed items. First, we present the results for posttest Item 3 (see Fig. 1). Most students of the intervention group (63.7%) based their advice on data from research by Consumer Report among 400 participants, and only the minority based their advice on the personal experiences of Mrs. Jones’ friends (35.6%). However, in the comparison group, we observed an inverse situation. Here, the majority of the students based their advice on the experiences of the friends (71.0%), and only a few students based their opinion on the Consumer Report survey (5.9%). A chi-squared test on the distribution over levels in percentages between both groups confirmed a significantly higher score for the intervention group (χ2 (2) = 80.84, p < 0.0005). The results show that students who attended the LT drew their conclusion on data-based claims. Preferring statistical information over personal intuition and bias is an important step toward statistical inference.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Students’ achievements on posttest item 3, taken from Watson and Callingham (2004)

Second, we present the results on posttest item 8, a newly designed item (see Fig. 2). Most students from both groups noted one specific value as their estimate of the sample result. In the intervention group, more students considered sampling variability (28.1%) than in the comparison group (19.3%). In the comparison group, almost one-third of the answers (30.3%) did not match their answer given in item 7, while for the intervention group, only a smaller one-sixth (15.7%) did so. A chi-squared test on the distribution over levels confirmed a significantly higher score for the intervention group (χ2 (2) = 6.57, p < 0.05).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Students’ achievements on posttest item 8, newly designed item

Third, we regard the results for posttest item 9, a newly designed posttest item related to items 7 and 8 (see Figs. 2 and 3). Most students in the comparison group (63.9% for levels 0–1) focused on the context, without referring to the data from their sketched graph in posttest item 7 or their average in item 8, and without taking variability into account. For the intervention group, most students (62.6% for levels 2–5) did relate data from their graph or average to the context; however, half of these students (31.8%, level 2) argued a specific sample value without taking variability into account. A chi-squared test on the distribution over levels confirmed a significantly higher score for the intervention group (χ2 (5) = 28.19, p < 0.0005). As such, the results for items 8 and 9 show that students who were taught using the LT performed better on making data-based claims with reference to statistical information and accompanied by probabilistic reasoning.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Students’ achievements on posttest item 9, newly designed item

Results on Students’ Learning Progression

This section describes whether the supporting indicators for LT steps 1 to 8 were observed in students’ worksheets (see Table 5). Column 3 presents the percentage of students that correctly elaborated the indicator in their work. In the following part, we highlight results from LT steps 2, 3, 4, and 7, which provided us with insight into how each of these LT steps fostered or hindered the students’ learning process.

Table 5 Overview of results for LT steps 1 to 8

Step 2: Visualizing the Black Box Sampling Distribution to Make Inferences (Categorical Data)

In LT step 2, for indicator 2a, most students (91%) drew a correct visualization of the expected sampling distribution as a global bell-shaped curve with a peak at 30. These students’ drawings could be divided in four types (see Fig. 4). For indicator 2b, 99% of the students correctly determined the probability of a sample result of more than 34 orange marbles based on the sampling distribution given. Students’ drawings and statements demonstrate their emerging understanding of the sampling distribution—that is, understanding the visualization of the frequency distribution from repeated sampling and using the distribution as a model for determining the probability of certain sample results—in the context of a black box. Although high deviating results were overestimated in some students’ drawings and incorrect local peaks appeared, most students correctly drew a bell-shaped curve with a peak at the population proportion. Furthermore, most students correctly determined the probability of a certain range of sample results using the sampling distribution given. In a short period of time, after just one lesson, students were able to draw and interpret the (expected) sampling distribution. We assume that the physical experiments from LT step 1, combined with classroom exchange and discussion, facilitated students for LT step 2. As such, we consider LT steps 1 and 2 as essential.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Four types of correct student drawings (N = 243) of the expected results of repeated sampling (100,000 repetitions) with sample size 40 in a sampling distribution, with percent per type of elements to foster students’ learning progress

Step 3: Modeling a Black Box to Make Inferences (Categorical Data)

For step 3, the findings evidence that 77% of the students were able to use statistical modeling in TinkerPlots to determine most likely sample results within the context of a black box. The other 23% of the students incorrectly noted a vague or deterministic answer, for example: “According to TinkerPlots probably more orange than yellow marbles” or “A sample will contain 30 orange and 20 yellow.” Teachers noted that most students independently deployed the required statistical modeling processes in TinkerPlots. Only a few students needed help in applying the correct digital techniques or interpreting the displays on their screen, for example, the sample and sampling distributions. Teachers’ feedback for those students mainly consisted of referring to the physical black box experiment and TinkerPlots instruction sheet, in particular by making explicit the similarities between the experiment and the TinkerPlots environment. As such, the initial physical black box activities in LT step 1 and 2 proved meaningful for introducing statistical modeling activities in step 3.

Step 4: Modeling Real-Life Contexts to Make Inferences (Categorical Data)

In LT step 4, for indicator 4a and 4b, most students were able to use statistical modeling for interpreting the effect of larger sample size (98%) and for probabilistic reasoning in real-life contexts (83%). We observed more context-independent terminology than in steps 1 to 3, as students’ statements involved samples, sample size, probability, and variability. Teachers indicated that in the first of three lessons in step 4, about one-third of the students had difficulties applying statistical modeling in new contexts. Teachers’ instruction with reference to the black box context worked well for those students with problems. During lessons 2 and 3 of step 4, these difficulties hardly occurred. Teachers mentioned that students were inclined to refer back to the black box context in their (verbal) reasoning while working on their tasks with real-life contexts.

For indicators 4c and 4d, 73% of the students substantiated their statement with data found by statistical modeling in TinkerPlots. Of all students, 31% correctly stated that the school management can conclude that the breakfast habits of pupils are improved for unlikely high sample results—that is, for sample results above 80—and 42% incorrectly mentioned improvement for results higher than the common ones of 70, based on their TinkerPlots data found. Of all students, 27% did not refer to their TinkerPlots data found (see Fig. 5).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Percent of students per category of answers on worksheet 4 task 18

Students’ inferences within new real-life contexts accompanied by more sophisticated probabilistic reasoning—that is, more context-independent language and statistical terminology—confirmed their emerging understanding of key concepts. Students used their simulated sampling distribution as a model for probabilistic reasoning in real-life contexts, which is an important step toward emergent modeling. Regarding indicator 4d, using the sampling distribution to determine at what sample results it is likely that a given model can be rejected—an informal approach of hypothesis testing—appeared difficult for students. Although from steps 1 to 3, students were familiar with sampling variability, and they did not transfer this knowledge to their claim and tend to use the deterministic approach that any sample proportion found, higher than the population proportion, indicates a change of population. These results confirm earlier studies about students’ difficulties in understanding hypothesis testing (Stalvey et al., 2019). Nevertheless, 30% of the students correctly indicated when a given model should be rejected.

Step 7: Modeling a Black Box to Make Inferences (Numerical Data)

In LT step 7, for indicator 7a: making inferences about the population distribution, students tended to reflect the shape of one sample distribution found in TinkerPlots directly to the population (see Fig. 6). However, when using a small sample size, a strict reflection often results in an incorrect irregular shape of the expected population distribution. Sample distributions for small sample sizes are less stable—sometimes even called dancing distributions—than for larger sample sizes. About half of the students (52%) compensated for these irregular shapes by comparing several (simulated) sample distributions, probably based on their experiences in LT steps 5 and 6—concerning classroom exchange and discussion of varying sample distributions found from the physical black box experiment.

Fig. 6
figure 6

Percent of students per category of answers on worksheet 7 tasks 1 and 8

For indicator 7b, regarding the effect of sample size on the expected population distribution, most students (78%) correctly stated that the distribution from a larger sample better reflects the population distribution. Most of these students (73%) explicitly mentioned that larger sample sizes lead to more stable distributions: less variability, smoother bell -curve, a peak at the population mean, and fewer local peaks; the other 27% of these students stated that a larger sample contains more information which results in a “bigger” distribution: has a wider range of results and higher bars. For 22% of the students, we found incorrect statements, for example: “The distributions for small and large sample sizes are quite similar.” For making inferences about the population mean using small and large samples, most students (69%) stated that a larger sample leads to a better estimate of the population mean: more stable, precise, and reliable. The other 31% stated that for the expected population mean, using small or large samples sizes was quite similar.

Regarding indicator 7e, most students (68%) had difficulties determining the probability of certain sample results. Students’ problems mainly consisted of confusing the sample and sampling distribution. For example, when students were asked to determine the probability of a sample mean below 1.55 m, the students tended to refer to their simulated sample distribution instead of the sampling distribution; we also observed the other way around, when students were asked to determine the probability that a person’s height is below 1.55 m. We assume that the emphasis on three distributions—that is, sample, population, and sampling distribution—in LT steps 5 and 6 caused confusion.

Overall, teachers explicitly mentioned that the black box as guiding activity through the learning trajectory was clear and useful, especially the strong similarities between the physical black box and statistical modeling in TinkerPlots. Furthermore, teachers described the black box as a concrete, engaging activity that is free of bias—meaning not related to students’ personal preference or prior knowledge. The learning of digital techniques for using TinkerPlots in a short period of time took some time and effort. Teachers indicated that investing in these techniques was worthwhile, and that most students deployed the techniques rather easily.

Conclusion and Discussion

This article reports on a design study that aimed for a theoretically and empirically underpinned design of an LT for introducing statistical inference in Grade 9. We addressed several aspects involved in design research on LTs as advised by Duschl et al. (2011). To evaluate the designed LT, we analyzed the progression made by 267 students. First, the analysis of the posttest results indicates that students’ understanding of statistical inference as addressed in the coupled LT steps—in LT steps 1 and 5 on using samples, in LT steps 2 and 6 on visualizing distributions, in LT steps 3 and 7 on repeated sampling and effect of sample size, and in LT steps 4 and 8 on solving real-life problems—was significantly higher among students who took part in the LT than among students who followed the regular curriculum. These results demonstrate a higher score for all eight learning steps and, with that, a deeper understanding of the statistical concepts offered in each step. As such, it appears that all eight steps combined led to students’ higher performance on statistical inference. Second, the analysis of students’ worksheets, accompanied by teachers’ and researcher’s notes, confirms that all eight steps of the learning trajectory combined contributed in fostering students’ learning. In addition to developing the statistical concepts addressed within each learning step, we also observed progress across the eight successive learning steps—for example, in using more abstract statistical terminology, data-based reasoning, and context-independent use of statistical concepts and models. As such, the results empirically substantiate the theoretically designed learning trajectory.

Although research shows that reasoning and interpreting sampling distributions is difficult (Batanero et al., 1994; Castro Sotos et al., 2007; Chance et al., 2004), the findings show that students can develop key concepts of statistical inference—sample, variability, and distributions—in a short period of time by using the black box sampling as guiding activity. Starting from LT steps 1 and 2, students developed an emerging understanding of the sampling distribution, initially as a visualization or model of their results, and gradually as a model for determining the probability of certain sample results. The strong similarity between the physical black box activities and the modeling activity in the digital environment of TinkerPlots facilitated the connection of the model to the real world (Konold & Kazak, 2008; Patel & Pfannkuch, 2018). In following LT steps, the black box served as a guiding paradigm for students’ reasoning and teacher instructions about key concepts, in particular while modeling real-life phenomena.

Based on the promising results of this study into an LT for introducing statistical inference—designed on the basis of current ideas and theories in this area—we identify the following design heuristics as useful. First, the learning activities should be placed in a context that allows students to develop statistical concepts directly related to the learning goals of the LT—that is, a context that is recognizable to students, engaging, activating, and representative for the concepts at stake. Second, although activities may focus on specific statistical concepts, they should be viewed within the broader perspective of the entire statistical investigation cycle. Here, it is essential that students go through this cycle repeatedly, using different contexts with increasing levels of abstraction and complexity. Third, visual and enactive similarity between material and digital sources must be ensured for performing statistically identical procedures. Fourth, explorative and iterative activities with simulation software should be embedded to facilitate the development of context-independent conceptual understanding. Fifth, activities should be structured to support learners in developing a model of their concrete statistical activity that can then be used as a model for a network of statistical concepts and relationships.

However, when higher order thinking activities were addressed in the LT, such as informal hypothesis testing or reasoning about population distributions, we saw confusion among students. Apparently, more time and more iteration are needed to anchor the key concepts before proceeding to more complex statistical concepts and ideas. We therefore suggest in Sequence II—steps 5 to 8—to focus on repeated sampling using the sample mean and to omit making inferences about the population distribution. In this way, the key concepts for statistical inference from Sequence I that emerge from the sample proportion of categorical data for repeated sampling can be further elaborated in Sequence II by using the sample mean of numerical data.

To address more complex statistical concepts in a follow-up LT, repeated sampling with a black box (or boxes) may also be used as guiding activity. With regard to hypothesis testing, which is difficult for many students (Stalvey et al., 2019), a hypothesis concerning the black box content can be used to introduce the idea of hypothesis testing. For example, by providing a physical black box filled with marbles and letting students test whether the given proportion is likely to be true. This also holds for other statistical concepts and ideas, such as determining the critical area and comparing groups, where the black box provides opportunities for engaging and guiding activities.

Concerning the use of digital technology in the LT, investigating in learning to use a digital tool—which took time and effort from both teachers and students—appeared fruitful for students’ understanding of statistical inference. The digital techniques for using the tool enabled students to identify context-independent patterns in action that seemed to facilitate the transition toward emergent modeling. This transition was reflected in students’ worksheets when they referred to similar previous technical actions and in students’ terminology that evolved from concrete terms to more abstract statistical terminology, for example from the term “viewing window” to “sample.” The development of a statistical vocabulary is essential for students’ understanding of concepts (Watson & Kelly, 2008).

The results of this study can be positioned within the findings of our larger study. The findings from the larger study using all assessment items of the pre- and posttest indicate that the LT also stimulated other domains of statistical literacy (Van Dijke-Droogers et al., submitted). These findings suggest that the current Dutch pre-10th grade curriculum can be enriched with informal statistical inference; we assume that this also holds for other countries with a focus on descriptive statistics in lower secondary mathematics curricula.

Of course, this study comes with some limitations. Teachers’ implementation of the LT varied, for example in the amount of teacher guidance and instruction during the teaching sequence. These differences were visible in students’ worksheets, with the reasoning of students with the same teacher being more or less similar. Furthermore, we encountered practical limitations during the intervention, such as difficulties with installing TinkerPlots on the school’s computer network and lesson shortening due to extremely high temperatures. The installation problems caused some delay but did not affect our study. Due to the lesson shortening, we collected 224 completed worksheets in Sequence II, instead of the 267 in Sequence I.

On a final note, the findings suggest that curricula with a strong descriptive focus can be enriched with an inferential focus—at least for this type of student population—with the benefit of students learning more about inference, but not less about descriptive statistics. We recommend that educators and researchers involved in the design of teaching materials consider the embedding of black box activities combined with statistical modeling, to anticipate subsequent steps in the students’ statistics education.