Skip to main content
Log in

Conditional inference and advanced mathematical study: further evidence

  • Published:
Educational Studies in Mathematics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this paper, we examine the support given for the ‘theory of formal discipline’ by Inglis and Simpson (Educational Studies Mathematics 67:187–204, 2008). This theory, which is widely accepted by mathematicians and curriculum bodies, suggests that the study of advanced mathematics develops general thinking skills and, in particular, conditional reasoning skills. We further examine the idea that the differences between the conditional reasoning behaviour of mathematics and arts undergraduates reported by Inglis and Simpson may be put down to different levels of general intelligence in the two groups. The studies reported in this paper call into question this suggestion, but they also cast doubt on a straightforward version of the theory of formal discipline itself (at least with respect to university study). The paper concludes by suggesting that either a pre-university formal discipline effect or a filtering effect on ‘thinking dispositions’ may give a better account for the findings.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The inference-type of an inference is defined as either ‘affirmative’ or ‘denial’ depending on the valence of the conclusion drawn from the non-negated conditional ‘if p then q’.

  2. The symbol ‘\(\neg\)’ here should be read as ‘not’.

  3. In the example above, ‘R’ is a more opaque representation of p than ‘\(\neg\)A’, and so, it is harder to see its relevance to the conditional ‘if \(\neg\)A then 3’.

  4. The advanced level (A level) is the qualification taken by 18-year-old school-leavers in England and Wales. It is marked on a seven-point scale from A (highest) to G (lowest), and each candidate typically studies three (or sometimes four) subjects. In 2007, around 3% of 18-year-olds across England and Wales achieved AAA or higher.

  5. An explanation of why this task is particularly appropriate for investigating conditional inference behaviour is given by Inglis and Simpson (2008).

References

  • Baron, J. (1988). Thinking and deciding. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bramall, S., & White, J. (Eds.) (2000). Why learn maths? London: Institute of Education, University of London.

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Feinstein, J. A., & Jarvis, W. B. G. (1996). Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 197–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coffield, F., Moseley, D., Hall, E., & Ecclestone, K. (2004). Learning styles and pedagogy in post-16 learning: A systematic and critical review. London: Learning & Skills Research Centre.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drysdale, M. T. B., Ross, J. L., & Schulz, R. A. (2001). Cognitive learning styles and academic performance in 19 first-year university courses: Successful students versus students at risk. Journal of Education for Students Placed at Risk, 6, 271–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. St. B. T. (2007). Hypothetical thinking: Dual processes in reasoning and judgement. Hove: Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. St. B. T., Clibbens, J., & Rood, B. (1995). Bias in conditional inference: Implications for mental models and mental logic. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48(A), 644–670.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. St. B. T., & Handley, S. J. (1999). The role of negation in conditional inference. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52A, 739–769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gill, P. (2003). Mathematics. In J. White (Ed.), Rethinking the school curriculum (pp. 104–116). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregorc, A. F. (1979). Learning/teaching styles: Potent forces behind them. Educational Leadership, 36, 234–237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heim, A. W. (1968). AH5 group test of intelligence. London: National Foundation for Educational Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inglis, M., & Simpson, A. (2008). Conditional inference and advanced mathematical study. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 67, 187–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kolb, D. A. (1984). Experiential learning: Experience as the source of learning and development. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason, J., Burton, L., & Stacey, K. (1982). Thinking mathematically. London: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moutsios-Rentzos, A., & Simpson, A. (2005). The transition to postgraduate study in mathematics: A thinking styles perspective. In H. L. Chick & J. L. Vincent (Eds.), Proceedings of the 29th international c onference on the psychology of m athematics education (Vol. 3, pp. 329–336). Melbourne, Australia: IGPME.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newstead, S. E., Handley, S. J., Harley, C., Wright, H., & Farrelly, D. (2004). Individual differences in deductive reasoning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 33–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riding, R. (1991). Cognitive styles analysis. Birmingham: Learning and Training Technology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riding, R., & Agrell, T. (1997). The effect of cognitive style and cognitive skills on school subject performance. Educational Studies, 23, 311–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schroyens, W., Schaeken, W., & d’Ydewalle, G. (2001). The processing on negations in conditional reasoning: A meta-analytical case study in mental model and/or mental logic theory. Thinking and Reasoning, 7, 121–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schroyens, W., Schaeken, W., Fias, W., & d’Ydewalle, G. (2000). Heuristic and analytic processes in propositional reasoning with negatives. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 26, 1713–1734.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sfard, A. (1998). A mathematician’s view of research in mathematics education: An interview with Shimson A. Amitsur. In A. Sierpinska & J. Kilpatrick (Eds.), Mathematics education as a research domain: A search for identity (Vol. 2, pp. 445–458). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, A. (2004). Making mathematics count: The report of Professor A drian Smith’s inquiry into post-14 mathematics education. London: The Stationery Office.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanic, G. M. A. (1986). The growing crisis in mathematics education in the early twentieth century. Journal for Research in Mathematlcs Education, 17, 190–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1999). Who is rational? Studies of individual differences in reasoning. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1998). Individual differences in rational thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 161–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 23, 645–726.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1924). Mental discipline in high school studies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 15, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zazkis, R., & Gadowsky, K. (2001). Attending to transparent features of opaque representations of natural numbers. In: A. Cuoco (Ed.), The roles of representation in school mathematics (pp. 146–165). Reston: NCTM.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zazkis, R., & Liljedahl, P. (2004). Understanding primes: The role of representation. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 35, 164–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Matthew Inglis.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Inglis, M., Simpson, A. Conditional inference and advanced mathematical study: further evidence. Educ Stud Math 72, 185–198 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-009-9187-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-009-9187-z

Keywords

Navigation