Skip to main content
Log in

Example-Based Learning in Heuristic Domains: A Cognitive Load Theory Account

  • Review Article
  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

One classical instructional effect of cognitive load theory (CLT) is the worked-example effect. Although the vast majority of studies have focused on well-structured and algorithmic sub-domains of mathematics or physics, more recent studies have also analyzed learning with examples from complex domains in which only heuristic solution strategies can be taught (e.g., troubleshooting, mathematical proving). Is learning by such examples also effective, and do the same instructional design principles apply? We discuss the main findings of an own research program and of related studies that addressed such questions. We found that CLT’s basic design principles also hold true for heuristic domains: Reduce extraneous load by employing examples, maximize germane load by fostering self-explanations, prevent cognitive overload by pretraining in the case of difficult learning materials, and by focusing attention on the most relevant aspects. Other typical CLT assumptions (e.g., better provide information for novice learners) were not confirmed in its generality. The present findings extend the applicability of CLT but also identify some potentially too simplistic assumptions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Atkinson, R. K., Derry, S. J., Renkl, A., & Wortham, D. W. (2000). Learning from examples: instructional principles from the worked examples research. Review of Educational Research, 70, 181–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, R. K., Renkl, A., & Merrill, M. M. (2003). Transitioning from studying examples to solving problems: effects of self-explanation prompts and fading worked-out steps. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 774–783. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.774.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1987). The psychology of written composition. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berthold, K., Eysink, T. H., & Renkl, A. (2008). Assisting self-explanation prompts are more effective than open prompts when learning with multiple representations. Instructional Science (in press) doi:10.1007/s11251-008-9051-z.

  • Braaksma, M. A. H., van den Bergh, H., Rijlaarsdam, G., & Couzijn, M. (2001). Effective learning activities in observation tasks when learning to write and read argumentative texts. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 1, 33–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braaksma, M. A. H., Rijlaarsdam, G., & van den Bergh, H. (2002). Observational learning and the effects of model-observer similarity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 405–415. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.94.2.405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, T., Ayres, P., & Sweller, J. (2005). The impact of sequencing and prior knowledge on learning mathematics through spreadsheet applications. Educational Technology Research and Development, 53, 15–24. doi:10.1007/BF02504794.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, A., Brown, J. S., & Newman, S. E. (1989). Cognitive apprenticeship: teaching the crafts of reading, writing, and mathematics. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.), Knowing, learning, and instruction (pp. 453–494). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., Loewenstein, J., & Thompson, L. (2003). Learning and transfer: a general role for analogical encoding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 393–408. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.2.393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerjets, P., Scheiter, K., & Catrambone, R. (2006). Can learning from molar and modular worked examples be enhanced by providing instructional explanations and prompting self-explanations. Learning and Instruction, 16, 104–121. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.02.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greer, B. (1997). Modeling reality in mathematics classrooms: the case of word problems. Learning and Instruction, 7, 293–307. doi:10.1016/S0959-4752(97)00006-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hilbert, T. S., & Renkl, A. (2008). Learning how to use a computer-based concept-mapping tool: Examples help. Computers in Human Behavior (in press).

  • Hilbert, T. S., Schworm, S., & Renkl, A. (2004). Learning from worked-out examples: the transition from instructional explanations to self-explanation prompts. In P. Gerjets, J. Elen, R. Joiner, & P. Kirschner (Eds.), Instructional design for effective and enjoyable computer-supported learning (pp. 184–192). Tübingen: Knowledge Media Research Center.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilbert, T. S., Renkl, A., Kessler, S., & Reiss, K. (2008a). Learning to prove in geometry: learning from heuristic examples and how it can be supported. Learning and Instruction, 18, 54–65. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.10.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hilbert, T. S., Renkl, A., Schworm, S., Kessler, S., & Reiss, K. (2008b). Learning to teach with worked-out examples: a computer-based learning environment for teachers. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 24, 316–332. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2729.2007.00266.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoogveld, A. W. M., Paas, F., & Jochems, W. M. G. (2005). Training higher education teachers for instructional design of competency-based education: product-oriented versus process-oriented worked examples. Teaching and Teacher Education, 21, 287–297. doi:10.1016/j.tate.2005.01.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hübner, S., Nückles, M., & Renkl, A. (2008). Instructional support to overcome strategy deficits in writing learning journals. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Kalyuga, S. (2008). Schema acquisition and sources of cognitive load. In J. L. Plass, R. Moreno, & R. Brünken (Eds.), Cognitive Load: Theory & Application. New York: Cambridge University Press.

  • Kalyuga, S., Ayres, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). The expertise reversal effect. Educational Psychologist, 38, 23–31. doi:10.1207/S15326985EP3801_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: an analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry based teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41, 75–86. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep4102_1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krause, U.-M., Stark, R., & Mandl, H. (2008). The effects of cooperative learning and feedback on e-learning. Learning and Instruction (in press). doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2008.03.003.

  • Mayer, R., & Moreno, R. (2003). Nine ways to reduce cognitive load in multimedia learning. Educational Psychologist, 38, 43–52. doi:10.1207/S15326985EP3801_6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (1994). Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem solving skills: a cognitive load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 122–133. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.86.1.122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F., & van Gog, T. (2006). Optimising worked example instruction: different ways to increase germane cognitive load. Learning and Instruction, 16, 87–91. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.02.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pollock, E., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2002). Assimilating complex information. Learning and Instruction, 12, 61–86. doi:10.1016/S0959-4752(01)00016-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A. (2002). Worked-out examples: instructional explanations support learning by self-explanations. Learning and Instruction, 12, 529–556. doi:10.1016/S0959-4752(01)00030-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A. (2005). The worked-out-example principle in multimedia learning. In R. Mayer (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 229–246). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A., & Atkinson, R. K. (2007). An example order for cognitive skill acquisition. In F. E. Ritter, J. Nerb, E. Lehtinen, & T. M. O’Shea (Eds.), In order to learn: how the sequence of topics influences learning (pp. 95–105). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A., Gruber, H., Weber, S., Lerche, T., & Schweizer, K. (2003). Cognitive Load beim Lernen aus Lösungsbeispielen. Zeitschrift fur Padagogische Psychologie, 17, 93–101. Cognitive load when learning from worked examples. doi:10.1024//1010-0652.17.2.93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A., Atkinson, R. K., & Große, C. S. (2004). How fading worked solution steps works—a cognitive load perspective. Instructional Science, 32, 59–82. doi:10.1023/B:TRUC.0000021815.74806.f6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rourke, A., & Sweller, J. (2008). The worked-example effect using ill-defined problems: learning to recognise designers’ style. Learning and Instruction (in press). doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2008.03.006.

  • Rummel, N., & Spada, H. (2005). Learning to collaborate: an instructional approach to promoting collaborative problem solving in computer-mediated settings. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 14, 201–241. doi:10.1207/s15327809jls1402_2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rummel, N., Spada, H., & Hauser, S. (2006). Learning to collaborate in a computer-mediated setting: observing a model beats learning from being scripted. In S. Barab, K. Hay, & D. Hickey (Eds.), Proceedings of the 7th International Conference of the Learning Sciences. (pp. 634–640). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoenfeld, A. H. (1985). Mathematical problem-solving. Orlando: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwonke, R., Renkl, A., Krieg, C., Wittwer, J., Aleven, V., & Salden, R. (2008). The worked-example effect: is it just an artefact of lousy control conditions? Computers in Human Behavior (in press).

  • Schworm, S., & Renkl, A. (2006). Computer-supported example-based learning: when instructional explanations reduce self-explanations. Computers & Education, 46, 426–445. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2004.08.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schworm, S., & Renkl, A. (2007). Learning argumentation skills through the use of prompts for self-explaining examples. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 285–296. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.99.2.285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, (2006). The worked example effect and human cognition. Learning and Instruction, 16, 165–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., & Cooper, G. A. (1985). The use of worked examples as a substitute for problem solving in learning algebra. Cognition and Instruction, 2, 59–89. doi:10.1207/s1532690xci0201_3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10, 251–296. doi:10.1023/A:1022193728205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., Kirschner, P. A., & Clark, R. E. (2007). Why minimally guided teaching techniques do not work: a reply to commentaries. Educational Psychologist, 42, 115–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tarmizi, R. A., & Sweller, J. (1988). Guidance during mathematical problem solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 424–436. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.80.4.424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Gog, T., Paas, F., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2006). Effects of process-oriented worked examples on troubleshooting transfer performance. Learning and Instruction, 16, 154–164. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.02.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Gog, T., Paas, F., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2008). Effects of studying sequences of process-oriented and product-oriented worked examples on troubleshooting transfer efficiency. Learning and Instruction, 18, 211–222. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2007.03.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (1990). Strategies for programming instruction in high school: program completion vs. program generation. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 6, 265–287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Sweller, J. (2005). Cognitive load theory and complex learning: recent developments and future directions. Educational Psychology Review, 17, 147–177. doi:10.1007/s10648-005-3951-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Kirschner, P. A. (2007). Ten steps to complex learning. A systematic approach to four-component instructional design. Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhu, X., & Simon, H. A. (1987). Learning mathematics from examples and by doing. Cognition and Instruction, 4, 137–166. doi:10.1207/s1532690xci0403_1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alexander Renkl.

Additional information

This research was partly funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG; Re 1040/4-1, 4-2, and 4-3).

This contribution reviews research on example-based learning in heuristic domains. Its main purpose is to propose five theses that are directly relevant for instructional design.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Renkl, A., Hilbert, T. & Schworm, S. Example-Based Learning in Heuristic Domains: A Cognitive Load Theory Account. Educ Psychol Rev 21, 67–78 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-008-9093-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-008-9093-4

Keywords

Navigation