Skip to main content
Log in

‘Folk Theories’ About the Causes of Insomnia

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study investigates ‘folk theories’ about the causes of insomnia. Participants with insomnia (n = 69) completed a qualitative and quantitative assessment of their folk theories. The qualitative assessment was to speak aloud for 1 min in response to: ‘What do you think causes your insomnia?’. The quantitative assessment involved completing the ‘Causal Attributions of My Insomnia Questionnaire’ (CAM-I), developed for this study. The three most common folk theories for both the causes of one’s own insomnia as well as insomnia in others were ‘emotions’, ‘thinking patterns’ and ‘sleep-related emotions’. Interventions targeting these factors were also perceived as most likely to be viable treatments. Seventy-five percent of the folk theories of insomnia investigated with the CAM-I were rated as more likely to be alleviated by a psychological versus a biological treatment. The results are consistent with research highlighting that folk theories are generally coherent and inform a range of judgments. Future research should focus on congruence of ‘folk theories’ between treatment providers and patients, and the role of folk theories in treatment choice, engagement, compliance and outcome.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ahn, W., Proctor, C. C., & Flanagan, E. H. (2009). Mental health clinicians’ beliefs about the biological, psychological, and environmental bases of mental disorders. Cognitive Science, 33, 147–182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baglioni, C., Lombardo, C., Bux, E., Hansen, S., Salveta, C., Biello, S., et al. (2010). Psychophysiological reactivity to sleep-related emotional stimuli in primary insomnia. Behavior Research and Therapy, 48, 467–475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bastien, C. H., Vallieres, A., & Morin, C. M. (2001). Validation of the insomnia severity index as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep Medicine, 2, 297–307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daley, M., Morin, C. M., LeBlanc, M., Gregoire, J. P., & Savard, J. (2009). The economic burden of insomnia: direct and indirect costs for individuals with insomnia syndrome, insomnia symptoms, and good sleepers. Sleep, 32(1), 55–64.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S. (2000). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia, PA: Taylor & Francis/Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edinger, J. D., Bonnet, M. H., Bootzin, R. R., Doghramji, K., Dorsey, C. M., Espie, C. A., et al. (2004). Derivation of research diagnostic criteria for insomnia: report of an American Academy of Sleep Medicine Work Group. Sleep, 27, 1567–1596.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Epley, N., Keysar, B., Van Boven, L., & Gilovich, T. (2004). Perspective taking as egocentric anchoring and adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 327–339.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Espie, C. A., Barrie, L. M., & Forgan, G. S. (2012). Comparative investigation of the psychophysiologic and idiopathic insomnia disorder phenotypes: Psychologic characteristics, patients’ perspectives, and implications for clinical management. Sleep, 35(3), 385.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • First, M. B., Spitzer, M. B., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1995). Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV Axis I disorderspatient edition (SCID-I/P, Version 2.0). New York: Biomedics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute.

  • Glass, C. R., Arnkoff, D. B., & Shapiro, S. J. (2001). Expectations and preferences. Psychotherapy, 38, 455–461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goethals, G. R., Messick, D. M., & Allison, S. T. (1991). The uniqueness bias: Studies of constructive social comparison. In J. Suls & T. A. Will (Eds.), Social comparison: Contemporary theory and research (pp. 149–176). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., & Meltzoff, A. (1997). Words, thoughts and theories. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hajak, G., Petukhova, M., Lakoma, M. D., Coulouvrat, C., Roth, T., Sampson, N. A., et al. (2011). Days-out-of-role associated with insomnia and comorbid conditions in the america insomnia survey. Biological Psychiatry, 70, 1063–1073.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halford, W. K., Keefer, E., & Osgarby, S. M. (2002). “How has the week been for you two?” Relationship satisfaction and hindsight memory biases in couples reports of relationship events. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 26, 759–773.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, A. G., & Bryant, R. A. (1999). A qualitative investigation of the organization of traumatic memories. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38, 401–405.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, A. G., McGlinchey, E., & Gruber, J. (2009). Toward an affective science of insomnia treatments. In A. Kring & D. Sloan (Eds.), Emotion regulation and psychopathology (pp. 427–446). Guilford.

  • Harvey, A. G., Stinson, K., Whitaker, K. L., Moskovitz, D., & Virk, H. (2008). The subjective meaning of sleep quality: A comparison of individuals with and without insomnia. Sleep, 31(3), 383–393.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iacoviello, B. M., McCarthy, K. S., Barrett, M. S., Rynn, M., Gallop, R., & Barber, J. P. (2007). Treatment preferences affect the therapeutic alliance: Implications for randomized controlled trials. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 194–198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kempton, W. (1986). Two theories of home heat control. Cognitive Science, 10, 75–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, N. S., & Ahn, W. (2002a). Clinical psychologists’ theory-based representations of mental disorders predict their diagnostic reasoning and memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 451–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, N. S., & Ahn, W. (2002b). The influence of naive causal theories on lay concepts of mental illness. American Journal of Psychology, 115, 33–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • King, M., Nazareth, I., Lampe, F., Bower, P., Chandler, M., Morou, M., et al. (2005). Impact of participant and physician intervention preferences on randomized trials: A systematic review. Journal of the American Medical Association, 293, 1089–1099.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kocsis, J. H., Leon, A. C., Markowitz, J. C., Manber, R., Arnow, B., Klein, D. N., et al. (2009). Patient preference as a moderator of outcome for chronic forms of major de- pressive disorder treated with nefazodone, cognitive behavioral analysis system of psychotherapy, or their combination. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 70, 354–361.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kring, A. M. (2009). Emotion regulation and psychopathology: A transdiagnostic approach to etiology and treatment. Guilford: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kring, A. M., & Werner, K. H. (2004). Emotion regulation and psychopathology. In P. Philippot & R. S. Feldman (Eds.), The regulation of emotion (pp. 359–385). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruger, J. (1999). Lake Wobegon be gone! The “below-average effect” and the egocentric nature of comparative ability judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 221–222.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kwan, B. M., Dimidjian, S., & Rizvi, S. L. (2010). Treatment preference, engagement, and clinical improvement in pharmacotherapy versus psychotherapy for depression. Behavior Research and Therapy, 48, 799–804.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lichstein, K. L., & Rosenthal, T. L. (1980). Insomniacs’ perceptions of cognitive versus somatic determinants of sleep disturbance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 105–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lombrozo, T. (2012). Explanation and abductive inference. In K. J. Holyoak & R. G. Morrison (Eds.), Oxford handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 260–276). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morin, C. M., Belleville, G., Bélanger, L., & Ivers, H. (2011). The insomnia severity index: Psychometric indicators to detect insomnia cases and evaluate treatment response. Sleep, 34, 601–608.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morin, C. M., Gaulier, B., Barry, T., & Kowatch, R. A. (1992). Patients’ acceptance of psychological and pharmacological therapies for insomnia. Sleep, 15, 302–305.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, G. L., & Medin, D. L. (1985). The role of theories in conceptual coherence. Psychological Review, 92, 289–316.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nickerson, R. S. (1999). How we know—and sometimes misjudge—what others know: Imputing one’s own knowledge to others. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 737–759.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E. (1991). Learned optimism. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E. (1995). The effectiveness of psychotherapy. The consumer reports study. American Psychologist, 50, 965–974.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Skre, I., Onstad, S., Torgersen, S., & Kringlen, E. (1991). High interrater reliability for the structured clinical interview for DSM-III–R axis I (SCID-I). Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 84, 167–173.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Swift, J. K., & Callahan, J. L. (2009). The impact of client treatment preferences on outcome: A meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65, 368–381.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vincent, N., & Lionberg, C. (2001). Treatment preference and patient satisfaction in chronic insomnia. Sleep, 24, 411–417.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H., & Gelman, S. A. (1988). Children’s understanding of the nonobvious. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 4). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. B., Gibbon, M., First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Davis, M., Borus, J., et al. (1992). The structured clinical interview for DSM-III-R (SCID): Multisite test-retest reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49, 630–636.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This project was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grant RO1MH079188. We are grateful to Kerrie Hein & Manon Lamy (Project Co-ordinators), Jennifer Kanady, Jason Lee, Kate Kaplan & Eleanor McGlinchey (Assessors), Lisa S. Talbot, Polina Eidelman, Simon Beaulieu-Bonneau & Émilie Fortier-Brochu (Therapists) and Nicole Short (Coder) for their assistance with this study.

Conflict of Interest

Dr. Morin serves on consulting/advisory boards for Merck, Valeant, Purdue, Novartis, Eli Lilly and the speaker’s bureau for Valeant and Merck. The other authors have no conflict of interest disclosures.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Allison G. Harvey.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Harvey, A.G., Soehner, A., Lombrozo, T. et al. ‘Folk Theories’ About the Causes of Insomnia. Cogn Ther Res 37, 1048–1057 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-013-9543-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-013-9543-2

Keywords

Navigation