Skip to main content
Log in

Rumination and Vegetative Symptoms: A Test of the Dual Vulnerability Model of Seasonal Depression

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The Dual Vulnerability Model of seasonal affective disorder proposes that the cognitive-affective symptoms of seasonal depression are the result of an interaction of a diathesis of cognitive vulnerability to depression and the stressor of seasonal vegetative change. Two studies examined this hypothesis employing a within-subject design with daily data on vegetative and cognitive-affective depressive symptoms. Study 1 included a subclinical sample and a trait measure of ruminative response style. Study 2 included a clinical sample and reports of actual ruminative thoughts and behaviors in response to fatigue. Results of mixed linear model analyses in both studies supported the hypothesis that rumination moderates the relationship between the vegetative symptoms and the cognitive-affective symptoms of seasonal depression. The extension of the model to other subtypes of depression is considered.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The effect of vegetative symptoms was modeled as a random effect, consistent with our hypothesis that their impact on cognitive-affective symptoms varies from person to person. Moderating variables terms were modeled as fixed effects. Maximum likelihood estimation was used so that the fits of the models with different fixed effects could be compared. The statistical significance of fixed effects was assessed by a t-test based on the estimate and its standard error. The statistical significance of the standard deviation of random effect variables was assessed by comparing the fit of the model to the model with the effect fixed (Pinheiro and Bates 2004).

  2. In both studies, all dependent and independent variables were examined for being approximately normally distributed. This was the case, except for the positively skewed cognitive-affective variable in study 1, which, therefore, was log-transformed. Time-varying independent variables were person centered; those that varied only across persons were grand mean centered. In both studies visual inspection of graphs of the dependent cognitive-affective variable suggested that linear and quadratic general trends over time, both varying from person to person (i.e., as random effects), should be accounted for. In both studies, models with these terms significantly improved model fit. Next, a first-order autoregressive error structure was added. In both studies this addition also significantly improved model fit (autocorrelation ranged from .224 to .296 across all the models reported). Consequently, all subsequent models representing our hypotheses included these effects and served as the base models for assessing further improvements in model fit. For simplicity of presentation, these terms are not shown in the equation above or reported further in the Results. Details are available from the authors.

  3. For ease of presentation, in Study 1 coefficients and standard errors have been multiplied by 1000.

  4. Models were fit regressing rumination-to-mood on rumination-to-fatigue and vice versa, including, as before, linear and quadratic time effects and autocorrelated errors. The within-subject correlations were computed based on the reduction in variance from models without the independent variable. The value reported is the mean of the two values obtained, .4306 and .4023.

References

  • Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Alloy, L. B. (1989). Hopelessness depression: A theory-based subtype of depression. Psychological Review, 96, 359–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., Text Revision). Washington, DC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 53–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blodgett, C. J., Reardon, A., & Young, M. A. (2001). Rumination as a mediator of the relationship between neuroticism and both seasonality and depression. In Presented at the Annual Meeting of the Society for Light Treatment and Biological Rhythms, Madison, WI.

  • Bryk, A., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1992). Hierarchical linear models. Newbury Park: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eastman, C. I., Young, M. A., & Fogg, L. F. (1998). Bright light treatment of winter depression, a placebo-controlled trial. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 883–889.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Endicott, J., & Spitzer, R. L. (1978). A diagnostic interview: The schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 35, 837–844.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Enggasser, J., & Young, M. A. (2007). Cognitive vulnerability to depression in seasonal affective disorder: Predicting mood and cognitive symptoms in individuals with seasonal vegetative changes. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 31, 3–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, S. B. G., & Eysenck, H. J. (1964). An improved short questionnaire for the measurement of extraversion and neuroticism. Life Sciences, 3, 1103–1109.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hardin, T. A., Wehr, T. A., Brewerton, T., Kasper, S., Berrettini, W., Rabkin, J., et al. (1991). Evaluation of seasonality in six clinical populations and two normal populations. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 25, 755–787.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Insightful Corporation. (2003). S-Plus 6.2 for windows. Seattle: Insightful Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jang, K. L., Lam, R. W., Harris, J. A., Vernon, P. A., & Livesley, W. J. (1998). Seasonal mood change and personality: An investigation of genetic comorbidity. Psychiatry Research, 78, 1–7.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kane, A., & Lowis, M. J. (1999). Seasonal affective disorder and personality, age, and gender. South African Journal of Psychology, 29, 124–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kasper, S., Wehr, T. A., Bartko, J. J., Gaist, P. A., & Rosenthal, N. E. (1989). Epidemiological changes in mood and behavior: A telephone survey of Montgomery County, Maryland. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 823–833.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Madden, P. A., Heath, A. C., Rosenthal, N. E., & Martin, N. G. (1996). Seasonal changes in mood and behavior: The role of genetic factors. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 47–55.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, E., Tarrier, N., & Gregg, L. (2002). The nature and timing of seasonal affective symptoms and the influence of self-esteem and social support: A longitudinal prospective study. Psychological Medicine, 32, 1425–1434.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, G., Allen, N. B., & Trinder, J. (2001). A longitudinal investigation of seasonal variation in mood. Chronobiology International, 18, 875–891.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, G. W., Hay, D. A., & Armstrong, S. M. (1995). Personality factors in seasonal affective disorder: Is seasonality an aspect of neuroticism? Personality & Individual Differences, 19, 613–617.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569–582.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: The 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 115–121.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Morrow, J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1993). Response styles and the duration of episodes of depressed mood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 20–28.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Parker, L. E., & Larson, J. (1994). Ruminative coping with depressed mood following loss. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 92–104.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pinheiro, J. C., & Bates, D. M. (2004). Mixed-effects models in S and S-plus. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, J. E., Gilboa, E., & Gotlib, I. H. (1998) Ruminative response style and vulnerability to episodes of dysphoria: Gender, neuroticism, and episode duration. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 22, 401–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rohan, K. J., Roecklein, K. A., Tierney Lindsey, K., Johnson, L. G., Lippy, R. D., Lacy, T. M., & Barton, F. B. (2007). A randomized controlled clinical trial of cognitive-behavioral therapy, light therapy, and their combination for seasonal affective disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 489–500.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rohan, K. J., Sigmon, S. T., & Dorhofer, D. M. (2003). Cognitive-behavioral factors in seasonal affective disorder. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 71, 22–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, N. E., Gerhardt, M., Sack, D. A., Skewrer, R. G., & Wehr, T. A. (1987). Seasonal affective disorder: Relevance for treatment and research in bullimia. In J. I. Hudson & H. G. Pie (Eds.), Psychobiology of bulimia. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, N. E., Sack, D. A., Gillin, J. C., Lewy, A. J., Goodwin, F. K., Davenport, Y., et al. (1984). Seasonal affective disorder: A description of the syndrome and preliminary findings with light therapy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 41, 72–80.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, L., & Young, M. A. (2000). Clustering of symptoms across the offset of seasonal affective disorder. Presented at the Annual Meeting of the Society for Light Treatment and Biological Rhythms, Evanston, IL.

  • Spasojevic, J., & Alloy, L. B. (2001). Rumination as a common mechanism relating depressive risk factors to depression. Emotion, 1, 25-37.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D. (1985). Psychological treatments for depression: How do they work. Behavioural Research and Therapy, 23, 157–165.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • White, T. M., & Terman, M. T. (2004). The global seasonality score reconsidered: Convergence on diagnosis of winter depression. Paper Presented at the Annual Meeting of the Society for Light Treatment and Biological Rhythms, Toronto.

  • Williams, J. B. W., Link, M. J., Rosenthal, N. E., Amira, L., & Terman, M. (1992). Structured interview guide for the hamilton depression rating scales – seasonal affective disorder version (SIGH-SAD). New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, M. A., & Azam, O. (2003). Ruminative response style and the severity of seasonal affective disorder. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 27, 223–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young, M. A., Blodgett, C., & Reardon, A. (2003). Measuring seasonality: Psychometric properties of the SPAQ and ISV. Psychiatry Research, 117, 75–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Young, M. A., Meaden, P. M., Fogg, L. F., Cherin, E. A., & Eastman, C. I. (1997). Which environmental variables are related to the onset of seasonal affective disorder? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 554–562.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Young, M. A., Watel, L. G., Lahmeyer, H. W., & Eastman, C. I. (1991). The temporal onset of individual symptoms in winter depression: Differentiating underlying mechanisms. Journal of Affective Disorders, 22, 191–197.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael A. Young.

Appendix: Factor Analysis of Visual Analogue Scale (VAS) Items

Appendix: Factor Analysis of Visual Analogue Scale (VAS) Items

Data for the factor analysis came from the 45 original participants in Study 1. Participants completed visual analogue scales (VAS; items are in Table A1) on 42–70 days (M = 51.6, SD = 7.7). This sample’s demographic characteristics were similar to those of the Study 1 subsample. First, a covariance matrix was calculated for each participant; then these matrices were averaged to yield a single covariance matrix representing the typical within-subject covariability. An exploratory principle components factor analysis with varimax rotation was conducted on the covariance matrix to assess which symptoms clustered together as they changed across time.

Between-subject factor analyses of SAD symptom data have found vegetative and cognitive-affective factors (Madden et al. 1996). In a within-subject factor analysis of data from 10 SAD patients with the same VAS variables in this study, Young and Schmitt (2000) found two similar factors, labeled energy-motivation (a subset of vegetative) and cognitive-affective, plus a doublet of the two appetite items that neither loaded on these factors nor generated a clear separate factor. Based on these results, we examined a two-factor model and excluded the two appetite items. (Models including the two appetite variables produced estimation and interpretation problems commonly found with doublet items.) This solution produced the expected energy-motivation and cognitive-affective factors. Examination of a three-factor model yielded the same cognitive-affective factor and the energy-motivation items split into a factor with the positively worded items and a factor with the negatively worded items We considered the separation of the energy-motivation items to represent method variance. We therefore adopted the two-factor, energy-motivation and cognitive-affective solution (Table A1). The energy-motivation factor accounted for 20.5% of the total variance the cognitive-affective factor accounted for 23.4% of the total variance. Six items (forgetful, difficulty making decisions, happy, trouble doing daily activities, irritable, and optimistic) did not unambiguously load on one factor.

Table A1 VAS items and factor loadings from the two-factor solution

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Young, M.A., Reardon, A. & Azam, O. Rumination and Vegetative Symptoms: A Test of the Dual Vulnerability Model of Seasonal Depression. Cogn Ther Res 32, 567–576 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-008-9184-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-008-9184-z

Keywords

Navigation