Introduction

Autistic children perceive and engage with the world in a different way to their non-autistic peers, including having different ways of communicating with others, having highly focused interests, and engaging in self-stimulating behavior (Williams & Roberts, 2018). These children may also experience mental health challenges, including difficulties with anxiety, mood, and somatization (Crane et al., 2019; O'Nions et al., 2018; Ozsivadjian et al., 2021), sensory overload, pressure to mask their differences (Higgins et al., 2021; Strömberg et al., 2022), and challenges with transitions or unexpected changes (Sterling-Turner & Jordan, 2007). Taken together, these factors may relate to elevated rates of internalizing and externalizing  behaviors (O'Nions et al., 2018). Internalizing behaviors include symptoms associated with anxiety, mood conditions, and somatization (Lindsey et al., 2020; Smith et al., 2021). We use the term ‘externalizing behaviors’ to refer to ‘behavior(s) of such an intensity, frequency or duration that the physical safety of the person or others may likely be placed in serious risk, or behavior which may limit the use of, or result in the individual being denied access to ordinary public facilities’ (Emerson, 2001, p. 44). Externalizing behaviors include behaviors that others may find challenging (e.g., aggression, non-compliance, tantrums) (Bader et al., 2015; Schiltz et al., 2018) and are conceptualized as observable behaviors that relate to the person’s external environment (McRae et al., 2019). Parents of autistic children may make extensive adaptations to daily routines in response to their child’s externalizing behaviors (O'Nions et al., 2018, 2020). These accommodations may help support the child to engage in essential daily activities (e.g., reducing uncertainty, material rewarding). However, some accommodations may inadvertently maintain or exacerbate the challenges that the child faces and perpetuate externalizing behaviors (Lucyshyn et al., 2004). The purpose of this review is to investigate what parenting practices are associated with lower or higher levels of externalizing behaviors in autistic children. Understanding these behaviors and associated factors, specifically parenting practices is vital given externalizing behaviors are associated with suicidal ideation, depressive and anxiety symptoms, challenging parent–child interactions, and negative impacts on academic performance, at least within neurotypical children (Kerr et al., 2013; Soto-Sanz et al., 2019) with emerging research suggesting similar findings for autistic children (La Buissonnière Ariza et al., 2021).

Parenting Styles and Parenting Behaviors in Parents of Autistic Children

Parenting refers to the coordination of behaviors, resources, and efforts that parents use to guide the growth, welfare, health, and behaviors of their children (Aguirre-Dávila et al., 2023). Parenting includes parenting styles and parenting behaviors. Parenting styles describe ‘how parents parent’ and have been defined along two broad dimensions: parental warmth and behavioral/psychological control (Lambrechts et al., 2011; Maljaars et al., 2014). Parental warmth includes parenting behaviors such as acceptance, involvement, sensitivity, and positive affect (also called positive parenting, see Maljaars et al., 2014; McRae et al., 2019). Parental behavioral control includes behaviors such as managing/controlling child behaviors via monitoring, setting limits and standards, strict discipline, adapting the environment, and parental psychological control includes more intrusive controlling behaviors such as inconsistent discipline, withdrawal of love and guilt induction (also called negative parenting in the literature) (Lambrechts et al., 2011; Maljaars et al., 2014). As a function of the two parenting dimensions of parental warmth and control, Baumrind’s Theory of Parenting Styles identified three parenting styles in parents of neurotypical children: permissive (high warmth and low control), authoritarian (low warmth and high control), and authoritative (high warmth and high control) (Baumrind, 1966, 2016). Thus, parenting styles are defined as a broad collection of behaviors expressed by the parent, which create an emotional environment for the child (Locke & Prinz, 2002). In recent research, mindful parenting has been introduced as an additional parenting style informed by mindfulness-based theory. It is based on Indian-Buddhist philosophy and defined as ‘paying attention to your child and your parenting in a particular way: intentionally, here and now, and non-judgmentally’ (Kabat-Zinn & Kabat-Zinn, 1997, p. 22). O’Nions et al. (2018) described other specific and observable parenting strategies (e.g., accommodating the child, modifying the environment), which are goal and response oriented to child behaviors (objective to reduce child behaviors or are responses to child behaviors). Taken together, parenting is a complex construct. Parenting research on neurotypical children suggests that it may be problematic to only include one type of behavior, style, or strategy as this may not encapsulate the full intricacy of parenting (Calders et al., 2019) or be representative of the larger population and in this case—the autistic community. Therefore, for the purpose of this review, the term parenting practices will be used, which is inclusive of the broader parenting styles, mindful parenting, and parenting behaviors that occur across parenting dimensions, such as warmth and control, and specific parenting strategies to support child behaviors.

Associations Between Parenting Practices and Externalizing Behaviors

Specific parenting practices (e.g., parental control) have been associated with higher child externalizing behaviors in autistic children. McRae et al. (2018) found that a parenting practice characterized by harsh and disengaged parenting, inconsistent discipline, the use of corporal punishment, and poor monitoring and supervision, predicted higher levels of child externalizing behaviors. Further, Madarevic et al. (2022) found that parenting practices characterized as criticizing and ignoring the child were associated with greater externalizing behaviors in autistic children. Of note, however, across this research is that only one study (O’Nions et al., 2020) used an autism-specific measure of parenting to explore the relationship. This impacts interpretation as measures that are not designed for autistic children may not adequately capture parenting experiences for this population or may result in elevated scores where autism characteristics are rated as externalizing behaviors, thus, inflating associations (see discussion in Adams et al., 2019).

Other parenting practices, including mindful parenting have been associated with reduced child externalizing behavior in autistic children. Singh et al. (2006) found that mothers' increased mindful parenting was associated with their child’s decreased externalizing behaviors (such as aggression and non-compliance) and increased maternal self-efficacy in a small-scale multiple baseline study with three parent–child dyads. Similarly, Ridderinkhof et al. (2018), reported reductions in child externalizing behaviors, changes in parenting skills, and overall increased parental mindfulness following a mindful parenting intervention in a sample of 23 parent–child dyads. Taken together, these results suggest increased mindful parenting, may be associated with lower levels of child externalizing behavior, at least within parent intervention research.

O’Nions et al.’s (2018) meta-synthesis explored qualitative data on parenting practices in response to externalizing behaviors in autistic children. The authors found a range of parenting strategies used by parents of autistic children to support their child’s behaviors, including accommodating the child, modifying the environment, providing structure and routine, supervision and monitoring, managing non-compliance with everyday activities, responding to externalizing behaviors, managing distress, maintaining safety, and analyzing and planning. This study found that accommodating the child’s needs appeared to be the most common strategy used by parents in response to externalizing behaviors. This review was limited to qualitative analysis of case studies, case series, and qualitative studies, and as such could not investigate the strength of associations between parenting practices and externalizing behaviors. To improve outcomes for autistic children and their parents, it would be beneficial to understand and synthesize the quantitative research on associations between parenting practices and externalizing behaviors of autistic children to inform future parent-focused interventions.

Aim and Research Questions

The primary aim of this review is to synthesize the current literature on the associations between parenting practices among parents of autistic children and child externalizing behaviors through a systematic quantitative literature review. Evidence for the mediating and/or moderating role of other parent variables (e.g., parenting stress) and child variables (child age and gender) on the association between parenting practices and child externalizing behaviors will also be explored. Given limited understanding within the autism field on child age and gender effects on parenting practices (Maljaars et al., 2014), the literature will also be reviewed to address this gap to gain an understanding of possible child age and child gender effects on parenting practices. Understanding potential mediators or moderators of associations between parenting practices and child behaviors may be of value to identify families at higher probability of challenges, or targets for supports (e.g., reduction of parent stress may facilitate more supportive parenting).

The research questions are:

  1. 1.

    What is the strength of association between parenting practices and child externalizing behaviors in autistic children?

  2. 2.

    What is the scope and quality of research examining the associations between parenting practices and child externalizing behaviors among samples of autistic children?

  3. 3.

    Do any parent or child variables have mediating or moderating effects on the association between parenting practices and child externalizing behaviors?

  4. 4.

    Does child age and/or sex/gender have mediating or moderating effects on the associations between parenting practices and child externalizing behaviors?

Method

The protocol for this review is registered on the PROSPERO database (registration number CRD42022268667). Variations to the protocol included moving from the terminology of ‘challenging behavior’ to externalizing behavior and ‘on the autism spectrum’ to autistic in line with preferred language by the autistic community (Monk, 2022). This systematic review was conducted in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta-Analyses checklist (PRISMA; Page et al., 2021).

Search Strategy and Study Selection

PROSPERO was first searched to check if other systematic reviews on this topic existed. The search strategy was then developed by the first author in consultation with the university librarian and reviewed by the co-authors. The search strategy had three main categories: the target population—autistic child; second target population and their parenting practices—parents of autistic children and their parenting styles and behaviors; and child behaviors—externalizing behaviors exhibited by the autistic child, see Supplementary Material Table 2 for full listing.

Eligibility Criteria

Studies were identified via electronic searches of PsycINFO, CINAHL, PUBMED, Scopus, and Web of Science. Inclusion criteria were: (1) Published in peer-reviewed journal, (2) Participants included parents of autistic children (no restrictions on autistic children’s age, i.e., adult children will be included), or subgroups of this population, (3) Included a quantitative measures of parenting practices and externalizing behaviors of the autistic child, (4) Cross-sectional or longitudinal design, and (5) published in English. No publication date restrictions. Exclusion criteria were: (1) Qualitative studies, (2) Studies published in a language other than English, (3) Studies that included non-human participants, (4) Studies that did not include parents and their autistic children as participants or did not report this group separately, (5) Systematic review and meta-analyses, and (6) No measure of parenting practices and/or child externalizing behaviors.

Quality Assessment

Quality appraisal of selected studies was conducted using the McMaster Quality appraisal tool—McMaster Critical Review Form for Quantitative Studies (Law et al., 1998), which included: study purpose clarity, literature review relevance, appropriateness of study design, sampling, outcome measure reliability and validity, results analysis and interpretation. Inter-rater was conducted by a second reviewer (see Acknowledgements) rating 20% of the articles (see Supplementary Material Table 4) which resulted in 94% agreement.

Data Extraction and Management

Data were extracted and managed by the first author within Covidence (Covidence, 2022). Information extracted included: parenting practices and child externalizing behaviors and strength of associations between constructs. Where reported, data (measure, impact) on moderators and mediators of this association by other parent variables (e.g., parenting stress effects) and child variables (age and gender/sex of child) were also extracted. Other data extracted included: authors, year/location, participants numbers, sex/gender, ethnicity, age of child and parents, child autism diagnosis, child co-occurring conditions, parent mental health status and/or diagnoses, socio-economic status, parent type, measures used, and effect sizes found.

Data Synthesis

Data synthesis involved creating categories addressing the research questions and collating the key information from eligible studies, see Table 1. Parenting practices were grouped into those that were associated with lower child externalizing behaviors, mixed outcomes, and higher levels of child externalizing behaviors.

Table 1 Characteristics of included studies

Results

Searches were conducted on 25/04/2022 and updated on 10/03/2023. A total of 28,988 (2022 search) + 2790 (2023 search) (see Fig. 1) studies were identified, 13,956 (2022 search) + 1671 (2023 search) after removal of duplicates, which were screened by title/abstract. This resulted in 138 (2022 search) + 7 (2023 search) full-text articles being assessed for eligibility through full-text review, of which 29 articles (2022 search) + 1 article (2023 search) met full criteria. Five studies were subsequently excluded as they did not explore the associations between parenting practices and child externalizing behaviors, part of the inclusion criteria. Therefore, in total, 30 studies met all inclusion criteria (see Fig. 1). See Table 1 for participant characteristics and study characteristics of included studies. A second reviewer (see acknowledgements) screened 10% of the titles and abstracts of the articles found (selected using a random number generator). There was 98.93% agreement on papers meeting the inclusion criteria and 100% agreement on papers meeting the exclusion criteria. Discrepancies were discussed with a third reviewer (author four) until consensus was reached. This suggested high reliability of the inclusion/exclusion criteria for selected studies. Overall agreement was substantial (Cohen’s κ = 0.72). Although, this review focused on externalizing behaviors in autistic children, data from studies that included a wide age range of children in their sample (e.g., 1–23.9 years) were retained (n = 3).

Fig. 1
figure 1

PRISMA flow chart of articles screened

Child Gender, Age, and Ethnicity Information

Child participants were predominantly male, with most studies (n = 29) reporting child gender in a binary format (one study did not report gender). Across these there were 2480 males, 770 females, and two other gender, and 17 no answer/declined, with an overall ratio of 3.4:1 male:female. The child age range varied from 2.5 to 24 years old. Only 19 out of the 30 included studies reported the race/ethnicity of participants (see Table 1), with the majority of participants categorized as White (approximately 86%). Of these 19 studies, 2 studies included 100% Asian participants, and another 2 studies had a similar ratio of Black and White participants.

Risk of Bias and Quality of Studies

The risk of bias of the included studies was assessed (Supplementary Material Table 4). All studies included were considered low risk for selective reporting, implying that all included outcome measures were analyzed and reported. Twenty-four studies used a cross-sectional design, six studies had a longitudinal design. Most studies used reliable measures and only three studies (Boonen et al., 2014; Maljaars et al., 2014; O’Nions et al., 2020) used autism-specific measures. The included studies had varied definitions for parenting practices and externalizing behaviors (see definitions). All studies reported that they had approval from an appropriate ethical body for data collection and informed consent was indicated.

Variability Across the Research

Definitions

The definition of child externalizing behaviors varied among the included studies with some studies (n = 6) (e.g., Portes et al., 2020; Shawler & Sullivan, 2017) including a range of behaviors (e.g., externalizing behaviors included internalizing behaviors within the definition, such as anxiety related symptoms), whereas others defining externalizing behaviors or specific externalizing behaviors (e.g., aggression) (e.g., Boonen et al., 2014; Storch et al., 2015). In terms of parenting practices, there were a range of definitions [i.e., parenting dimensions and behaviors (positive—warmth dimension, or negative behaviors—control dimension); parenting styles (authoritative, authoritarian, permissive); mindful parenting; specific parenting strategies (accommodation, reducing uncertainty)]. The definitions on mindful parenting practices were consistent in the five studies (i.e., Aydin, 2022; Beer et al., 2013; Cheung et al., 2019; Mills et al., 2022; Raulston et al., 2021) investigating mindful parenting. Definitions of parenting styles (authoritative, authoritarian, permissive) (e.g., Clauser et al., 2021; Greenlee et al., 2022) were consistent across the studies. Definitions of specific parenting behaviors (positive, negative, warm, supportive, lax, autonomy, control) (e.g., Bader & Barry, 2014; Brinkman et al., 2022; Dieleman et al., 2018; McRae et al., 2018) were similar across studies. Definitions of parenting strategies to support externalizing behaviors (e.g., reinforcement, reducing uncertainty, accommodation) (i.e., Boonen et al., 2014; Maljaars et al., 2014; O’Nions et al., 2020; Storch et al., 2015) differed across the studies, i.e., different operational definitions of parenting strategies were used, with differences observed even for specific strategies, e.g., O’Nions et al. (2020) defined accommodation as particular observable behaviors specific to parents of autistic children, whereas Storch et al. (2015) defined accommodation as behaviors adopted by parents to reduce child behaviors. Three of the four studies used the PBS-A measure to measure these parenting strategies, with the exception of Storch et al. (2015) who used the PAS scale.

Measurement

All 30 studies used self-report measures to measure parenting practices (see Supplementary Material Table 3 for parenting classification scheme) and parent reports to measure child externalizing behaviors. Most of the measures used in the included studies were not specifically designed/validated for parents of autistic children (n = 27/30 studies). Exceptions were O’Nions et al. (2020) who used autism-specific measures for both parenting practices (PSQ and PBS-Autism) and child behaviors (HSQ-Autism), and Maljaars et al. (2014) and Boonen et al. (2014) who used the PBS-Autism to measure parenting practices. Six studies reported a total/subscale child behaviors score (which included items that assessed internalizing behaviors as well). Two studies reported a total score of a measure evaluating externalizing behaviors only. Twenty two of 30 studies reported on child externalizing behaviors separately (i.e., subscales/composite scores).

Associations Between Parenting Practices and Externalizing Behaviors in Autistic Children

Parenting Practices Associated with Lower Levels of Externalizing Behaviors in Autistic Children

Mindful Parenting Practices

Five studies reported on mindful parenting practices measured using MIPQ, IM-P, and BMPS (Supplementary Material Table 7). All studies used a subscale of child externalizing behaviors, except Aydin et al. (2022) who used a total score of PKBS-2 scale. All five studies identified mindful parenting as negatively associated with child externalizing behaviors (r =  − .26 to − .42), i.e., higher levels of mindful parenting were associated with lower levels of child behavior (using composite measures of intensity and frequency). Four of the five studies recruited school-aged children, one study (Aydin et al., 2022) investigated this association in 3- to 6-year-old children. All five studies found similar findings with no age or gender differences across studies.

Parenting Practices and Mixed Associations with Externalizing Behaviors in Autistic Children

Parenting Practices: Parenting Styles (Authoritarian, Authoritative, Permissive)

Five studies reported on the association between parenting styles (e.g., as authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive styles) and child externalizing behaviors (frequency) using the PSDQ to measure parenting styles (see Supplementary Material Table 5). Results were consistent across studies for authoritarian parenting with higher levels of authoritarian parenting associated with higher levels of externalizing behaviors (frequency). Results for permissive parenting were relatively consistent with four studies finding higher levels of permissive parenting style associated with higher levels of externalizing behaviors (frequency). One exception, however, was Rahman and Jermadi (2021) who found no significant associations. Rahman and Jernadi (2021) study differed in that participants were Malaysian, and majority of the parents rated their parenting style as authoritative (highest M and use) and majority of the recruited participants had autistic children with a medical condition (87%), thus, adopting a parenting style with high warmth and high control, which may explain differing results.

Findings for authoritative parenting style were mixed. Greenlee et al. (2022) found in a large longitudinal study that higher levels of authoritative parenting at Time 2 in fathers were associated with lower levels of autistic child externalizing behaviors (frequency) at Time 3 one year later (r =  − .30**). However, in contrast, higher levels of authoritative parenting at Time 2 in mothers  were associated with higher levels of externalizing behaviors at Time 3 (r = .22*). This finding for mothers was similar to Portes et al. (2020), who found cross-sectionally that higher levels of authoritative parenting in mothers and fathers were  associated with higher levels of externalizing behaviors (frequency) in autistic children and Clauser et al. (2021) who found that authoritative parenting in mothers and fathers contributed to higher levels of externalizing behaviors (frequency). In contrast, two studies (Rahman & Jermadi, 2021; Ueda et al., 2020) found null effects. Of note, these latter two studies differed in that they used subscale scores instead of composite scores and included Malaysian and Japanese participants in contrast to participants from the USA or Brazil.

Parenting Practices: Parenting Strategies to Support Externalizing Behaviors

Four studies reported on parenting practices—parenting strategies to support their child’s externalizing behaviors (Supplementary Material Table 6). These included the broader concepts of—accommodating the child, modifying the environment, providing structure and routine, supervision and monitoring, managing non-compliance with everyday activities, responding to externalizing behaviors, managing distress, maintaining safety, and analyzing and planning, and autism-adapted behaviors. These are discussed together as they represent parenting strategies implemented to support their child’s externalizing behaviors. Three studies by Boonen et al. (2014), Maljaars et al. (2014), and Storch et al. (2015) found no associations between parenting practices (autism-adapted parenting, material rewarding, adapting the environment, stimulating the environment, and accommodation, respectively) and externalizing behaviors. O’Nions et al. (2020), found positive associations between parenting practices that included accommodation behaviors and four specific child externalizing behaviors (reactivity, demand-specific non-compliance, extreme demand avoidance, and social inflexibility non-compliance) with greater parental accommodation related to higher levels of child behaviors in both intensity and frequency (r = .41 to .53). Parenting practices such as reducing uncertainty had positive associations, i.e., were associated with higher levels, with three specific child externalizing behaviors (reactivity, extreme demand avoidance, and social inflexibility non-compliance) (r = .15 to .22), whereas reinforcement approaches had a positive association, i.e., associated with higher levels of, with only extreme demand avoidance, r = .13*. Overall, results suggested that higher levels of the specific parenting practices of accommodation are associated with higher levels of specific child externalizing behaviors (both intensity and frequency). Differences between the three studies might have been due to specific subscales used by O’Nions et al. (2020), which were context sensitive and autism adapted, with other studies using either the SDQ composite scores (Boonen et al., 2014; Maljaars et al., 2014) or CBCL composite scores (Storch et al., 2015), which were not specifically designed for autistic children. Mixed findings could also be due to different sample sizes, e.g., Boonen et al. (2014), Maljaars et al. (2014) and O’Nions et al. (2020) had relatively large sample sizes and recruited similar aged group children, whereas Storch et al. (2015) had a smaller sample size.

Parenting Practices: Warmth Dimension

Twelve studies (Supplementary Material Table 6) reported parenting practices along the warmth dimension, described as positive parenting, parenting involvement, communication, autonomy/ autonomy-supportive, satisfaction, and warm/supportive. Nine studies found no associations between warmth dimension parenting practices and externalizing behaviors. These studies had a mix of cross-sectional and longitudinal designs and used the following measures: APQ, PBS-A, PBS, PCRI, and Autonomy Support subscale. However, four studies found significant but contrasting associations. A cross-sectional study by Boonen et al. (2014) found that an element of warmth- positive parenting had a small association (r = .16) with more externalizing behaviors (frequency). A longitudinal study by Dieleman et al. (2017) found that an element of warmth- positive parenting, was associated one year later (r = .33) with more externalizing behaviors (frequency). In contrast, a longitudinal study Osborne et al., (2008) found another element of warmth involvement, was associated with fewer externalizing behaviors (frequency). Similarly, a cross-sectional study by Brinkman et al. (2022) found that overinvolvement was associated with higher levels of externalizing behaviors (frequency). Taken together, the relationship between parental warmth and child externalizing behaviors is unclear, potentially due to the way warmth is operationalized (e.g., positive parenting vs. involvement vs. overinvolvement) and the way it is measured impacting any association with externalizing behaviors.

Parenting Practices: Control Dimension

Eighteen studies (Supplementary Material Table 6) reported parenting practices along the control dimension, including negative parenting, negative control, psychological control, criticism, parenting overreactivity, limit setting, harsh/disengaged, discipline, rules, and harsh punishment. Across these studies, the association ranged between r =  − 0.6 and 0.63. Specifically, most (14 studies) found higher levels of controlling parenting practices (i.e., negative parenting, psychological control, parenting overreactivity, harsh/disengaged parenting, discipline, rules, and harsh punishment) were associated with higher levels of child externalizing behaviors (frequency). Only two studies (Davies et al., 2022; Osborne et al., 2008) found that higher levels of a specific control parenting practice–limit setting were associated with lower levels of child externalizing behaviors (frequency). These studies may have found different associations due to being the only studies that analyzed the specific parenting practice of limit setting (measured with PCRI Limit setting scale). It should be noted that both studies found a strong significant association; however, both studies were limited due to their measure of child behavior including internalizing behaviors, which likely heightened the strength of the associations between parenting practices and child behaviors. The 14 studies that reported a positive association  between parenting practices along the control dimension and child externalizing behavior used composite scores/subscales (n = 12/14) or total scores (n = 2/14) of externalizing behaviors. Two studies used total scores of a specific measure of externalizing behaviors only, i.e., Berliner et al. (2020) and Shawler and Sullivan (2017) used total scores of the ECBI measure and its associations with PS total and subscale scores.

Four studies reported on parenting practices—discipline with conflicting findings. Maljaars et al. (2014) found significant positive associations with externalizing behaviors (frequency) and higher levels of discipline (r = .25*). In contrast, O’Nions et al. (2020) found significant positive associations with two specific externalizing behaviors and inconsistent discipline using a different measure (demand-specific r = .22*** and extreme demand avoidance r = .24***) (frequency and intensity). Similarly, Berliner et al. (2020) found that lax discipline also had a significant positive effect on child externalizing behaviors (frequency), suggesting that more lax parenting behaviors were associated with higher levels of externalizing behaviors. However, a large study (n = 130) by Shawler and Sullivan (2017) on parenting laxness/lax discipline in parents of children aged between 3 and 11 years, reported a non-significant finding. It should be considered this latter study used a non-autism-specific measure to measure parenting practices. Further, Berliner et al. (2020) recruited participants with children between the ages of 2.5 and 4.5 years and may have also found an association due to a smaller sample size (i.e., potentially inflated associations).

Mediating and Moderating Effects

Parent Variables: Parenting Stress, Parenting Practices, and Parent Well-Being

Two studies (Cheung et al., 2019; Osborne et al., 2008) explored mediating and moderating effects of parenting stress on the association between parenting practices and child externalizing behaviors. Using longitudinal data, Osborne et al., (2008) found that parenting stress mediated the relationship between parenting practices at baseline and child externalizing behaviors at follow-up, such that higher levels of parenting practices (i.e., limit setting) were associated with lower levels of parenting stress, and in turn were associated with lower levels of child externalizing behaviors. Cheung et al. (2019), analyzed the mediating and moderating effects of parenting stress on the association between parent variables (mindful parenting, parents’ affiliate stigma, and parents’ mental well-being) and child externalizing behaviors. Parenting stress mediated the relationship between mindful parenting and child externalizing behaviors, such that higher levels of mindful parenting were associated with lower levels of parenting stress, and lower levels of parenting stress were associated with lower levels of child externalizing behaviors. The moderating effects of parenting stress on the above relationship was non-significant.

Parent variable moderators/mediators of relationship between parenting practices and child externalizing behaviors were rarely explored in the included studies. Instead, parenting practices were explored as the mediator/moderator on parent variables in six studies (see Supplementary Material Table 8). These studies found mediating and/or moderating effects of parenting practices, such that specific parenting practices had significant effects on child externalizing behaviors. Aydin (2022) found partial mediating effects of mindful parenting on the relationship between parental emotional regulation difficulties and child externalizing behaviors. Greenlee et al. (2022) found that authoritarian parenting styles mediated the relationship between parenting relationship satisfaction and child externalizing behaviors. Lindsey et al. (2020) found moderating effects of negative parenting practices, such that when negative parenting practices were higher, the association between autism characteristics and externalizing behaviors had a significant negative relationship; however, lower levels of negative parenting practices had no moderating effects, i.e., non-significant association between autism characteristics and externalizing behaviors. Osborne et al., (2008) found that the association between parenting stress at baseline and child externalizing behaviors at follow-up was no longer present, when the parenting practice of limit setting mediated the relationship. Shawler and Sullivan (2017) found that the parenting practice of discipline mediated the relationship between parenting stress and frequency of child externalizing behaviors, specifically parenting practice of harsh and overreactive behaviors mediated the relationship between parenting stress and frequency of child externalizing behaviors. One other study (Cheung et al., 2019) analyzed the moderating effects of parent well-being and found no significant effects.

Child Variables: Age and Gender

Child’s age was controlled for in eight studies, and child gender was controlled for in seven studies (i.e., these were entered stepwise and not analyzed as mediators or moderators). Among studies, there were generally no age or gender effects found, with one exception of Maljaars et al. (2014). They explored the effects of child age on parenting practices and found that specific parenting practices, including adapting the environment, were reported at a lower levels for adolescents as compared to children. Further, they found that material rewarding was lower for adolescents as compared to younger children. Only one of 30 studies analyzed the moderating effects of child gender on the association between parenting practices and child externalizing behaviors (Dieleman et al., 2017). The authors found that gender did not moderate the relationship between parenting practices (negative control and positive parenting) and child externalizing behaviors.

Discussion

The aim of this review was to synthesize the quantitative evidence on parenting practices and their relation to externalizing behaviors in autistic children. Key findings were: (1) Mindful parenting was associated with lower levels of child externalizing behaviors, such that as mindful parenting increased, child externalizing behaviors decreased; (2) There were mixed results for other parenting practices—parenting styles, parenting strategies, and practices across warmth and control dimensions; (3) There is significant variability in operationalizing and measuring key constructs, including child externalizing behaviors (e.g., total scores vs. subscales) and parenting practices (e.g., specific measure). The implications of these findings, limitations of the review, implications and conclusions are overviewed below.

All five studies on mindful parenting found that higher levels of mindful parenting were associated with lower levels of child externalizing behaviors. While these studies had a cross-sectional design and cannot determine causality, the broader intervention research on mindful parenting (Bögels et al., 2010; Burgdorf et al., 2019; De Bruin et al., 2015; Ridderinkhof et al., 2018), provides promising findings of a potentially causal relationship whereby increasing mindful parenting (via mindful parenting interventions) is associated with reduced child externalizing behaviors (Bögels et al., 2010; Burgdorf et al., 2019; De Bruin et al., 2015; Ridderinkhof et al., 2018). Furthermore, this research suggests that mindful parenting may be a potential target for interventions, considering that it is amenable to change and increases support for children as seen in previous research (e.g., Bögels et al., 2014; Ridderinkhof et al., 2018). Thus, there is potential for supporting autistic children via increasing their parent’s mindful parenting.

Autism literature on parenting practices—styles, parenting strategies, and behaviors across the warmth and control dimension showed mixed results on the associations with child externalizing behaviors. Although, all five studies that analyzed parenting styles used the same measure to analyze parenting styles, there were mixed findings between specific parenting styles [authoritative (high warmth and high control) and permissive (high warmth and low control)] and externalizing behaviors in autistic children. One common finding, however, was that authoritarian parenting styles (low warmth and high control) were associated with higher levels of child externalizing behaviors. This is consistent with research in neurotypical children and their parents (Calders et al., 2019). A meta-analysis by Pinquart (2017) with neurotypical children found bidirectional associations between externalizing behaviors and parenting behaviors of harsh and psychological control, as well as with behavioral control/authoritative parenting styles. Therefore, we cannot assume the direction of relationships, given this finding may be bidirectional or is based on associations and whether use of this parenting style exacerbates, challenges, or whether it is a helpful response in reaction to challenges autistic children face (e.g., difficulties with change or uncertainty that may lead to higher rates of externalizing behavior). The meta-analysis by Pinquart (2017) also found that higher levels of authoritarian parenting and parenting behaviors including harsh and psychological control, were associated with higher levels of externalizing behaviors concurrently and longitudinally, whereas parenting behaviors including warmth and behavioral control were associated with lower levels of externalizing behaviors concurrently and longitudinally. Autistic children may benefit from this parenting style or behaviors including behavioral control (e.g., require more proactive control from their parent in certain situations that they may find overwhelming/stressful), which may provide their children with more structure and/or to manage child externalizing behaviors, as seen in the neurotypical literature (Pinquart, 2017). This is consistent with the research on parenting training interventions for the autistic community (e.g., via parent-mediated interventions such as RUBI parent training), which provides promising findings of a potentially causal relationship whereby increasing proactive control parenting is associated with reduced child externalizing behaviors [see Randomized Controlled Trials (RCT) by Aman et al., 2009; Bearss et al., 2015]. This highlights the importance of parent-mediated interventions within the autism field to better support parents and their autistic children. Further, two studies that found no effects, used subscale scores instead of composite scores and included Malaysian and Japanese participants. Thus, measurement and/or cultural differences in parenting may impact associations observed between authoritative parenting and child externalizing behavior.

Parenting practices across the warmth dimension such as, warm/supportive parenting did not have any associations with child externalizing behaviors, with only two studies, one cross-sectional study (Boonen et al., 2014) finding a significant small positive association with child externalizing behaviors and a longitudinal study (Dieleman et al., 2017), finding a significant positive association between earlier child externalizing behaviors and positive parenting practices 1 year later. These results are in contrast to the broader parenting literature on neurotypical children that suggests protective effects of the warmth dimension on children’s behavior (e.g., Calders et al., 2019; Pinquart, 2017). It may be that autistic children respond differently to these practices or there are differences in parent–child synchrony impacting the benefits of parental warmth in certain situations (e.g., child requiring higher control/lower warmth in certain situations). Further changes over time indicate potential temporal effects, i.e., parents may adopt more warm parenting practices to address their child’s needs associated with their externalizing behaviors.

Findings on parenting practices—involvement/overinvolvement were mixed. Brinkman et al. (2022) found a significant positive association between the parenting practice of overinvolvement and child externalizing behaviors, whereas Osborne et al., (2008) found a significant negative association between the parenting practice of involvement and child externalizing behaviors. Osborne’s findings are consistent with the neurotypical literature, for example, Badri et al. (2014) found that the parenting practice of involvement (at school to help with transition from pre-school to kindergarten) was negatively associated with externalizing behaviors in neurotypical children. It is posited that differing findings between studies may indicate that the levels of parenting involvement and context might be important to consider, e.g., parent involvement during important transitions. However, higher levels of involvement may be unsupportive and become counter-productive and unsupportive for autistic children in some settings or at higher level as is seen in neurotypical development and may exacerbate externalizing behaviors.

Further, there was evidence of moderate to strong positive associations between parenting practices across the control dimension (i.e., negative control, psychological control, overreactivity) and child externalizing behaviors, suggesting that as these parenting practices increase, child externalizing behaviors also increase (e.g., Dieleman et al., 2018; Ventola et al., 2017). However, some mixed findings were obtained with two studies (Davies et al., 2022; Osborne et al., 2008) finding higher levels of the parenting practice—limit setting was associated with lower levels of child externalizing behaviors. Both these studies used a measure that included internalizing behaviors; therefore, it is likely that these associations may not be the same for externalizing behaviors only. However, these results on parenting practices—limit setting are similar to other studies with other neurodivergent samples (e.g., Austin et al., 2004), suggesting that in contrast to control broadly, that limit setting may be helpful and associated with lower levels of externalizing behaviors in neurodivergent children (e.g., provision of expectations and structure via limit setting/discipline). The seemingly differing associations between individual parenting practices and child externalizing behavior indicated between studies may reflect the importance of context. Context-dependent interventions for externalizing behavior have long been associated with positive outcomes with autistic children, including behavior analytic-informed intervention approaches (e.g., Triple-P Program, Tellegen & Sanders, 2014; Whittingham et al., 2009) and parent-mediated behavioral interventions (see review by Postorino et al., 2017). The theoretical foundation of such approaches with autistic children is in understanding the function of any given behavior, and modifying contingencies to address that function and shape behavior. In contrast, mindfulness-based approaches, such as those included within this review, do not seek to determine the function of specific child behavior, or to target this within the intervention. Rather, the theoretical underpinnings of mindfulness-based approaches are grounded in Buddhist cultural understandings of human behavior and focus on intrapersonal processes (and inter-personal processes in the case of mindful parenting) rather than contextual factors. Within the parenting context, these interventions are parent-focused, and target intrapsychic changes of the parent (e.g., parenting stress) to help parents be more effective in supporting child behavior. The pathways of change in child behavior resulting from these interventions may be quite different to behavioral analytic approaches. Future research that aims to determine both efficacy as well as mediators of change associated with different parenting interventions will elucidate the nuances associated with the complexity of parenting and its influences on child behavior outcomes.

Studies that reported on discipline/parenting laxness found mixed findings. These studies recruited different age groups of autistic children and used different measures to measure key constructs. Berliner et al. (2020) found inconsistent discipline/lax discipline was associated with an increase in externalizing behaviors. However, somewhat contradicting these findings, Maljaars et al. (2014), who used an autism-specific measure to assess parenting practices, found that higher levels of rules and discipline were associated with higher levels of externalizing behaviors. Contrasting further, Shawler and Sullivan (2017) found no associations between these parenting practices—laxness and child externalizing behaviors. These mixed findings appear to be consistent with the literature on discipline/parenting laxness in parents of neurotypical children, where Marchand-Reilly (2012) found that lax discipline (measured with PS scale) in fathers was a significant predictor of child externalizing behaviors (child age 4–8 years old), while Park et al. (2018) found that lax discipline (measured with APQ) was uniquely associated with child behaviors for mothers but not fathers (child age 9–12 years old). Future autism-based studies could reconcile these results by investigating the differences in caregiver type and related associations, the impacts of child age on these associations, as well as use autism-specific measures to ensure to accurately capture behaviors in autistic children.

Further, four studies explored parenting strategies to support their child externalizing behaviors including accommodation, reducing uncertainty, rewarding, and autism-adapted behaviors with mixed findings. Three studies (Boonen et al., 2014; Maljaars et al., 2014; Storch et al., 2015) found no associations with externalizing behaviors, whereas O’Nions et al. (2020) found specific parenting strategies of reinforcement approaches were associated with lower levels of child externalizing behaviors, whereas accommodation and reducing uncertainty were associated with increased levels of child externalizing behaviors. This is consistent with research on parenting accommodation and an increase in child obsessive compulsive related difficulties and internalizing behaviors (Lebowitz et al., 2012). The differences between the included studies (i.e., associations found when using a measure validated for the autistic community and to measure specific strategies to manage behaviors in autistic children) highlights the importance of further research using appropriate autism outcome measures for all constructs of interest to ensure they are sensitive to this community. Further, this also highlights the importance of further research on potential target (i.e., parenting practices) for interventions.

Two studies found mediating effects of parenting stress, such that parenting practices such as limit setting was associated with higher parenting stress, and in turn was related to higher child externalizing behaviors (Osborne et al., 2008). Whereas, mindful parenting was associated with lower levels of parenting stress and consequently lower levels of child externalizing behaviors (Cheung et al., 2019). These findings are consistent with research in neurotypical development (Jackson & Choi, 2018) that have similarly found parenting stress is associated with parenting practices that lead to greater child challenges. Considering that studies have consistently indicated that parents of autistic children experience considerably higher levels of stress as compared to other groups (Barroso et al., 2018), and that parenting stress is amenable to intervention in this group (e.g., Bögels et al., 2010), further research in this area could identify potential factors to target in interventions, as well as help better understand this relationship, and therefore, an important avenue for further research. Six studies found mediating/moderating effects of parenting practices on the relationship between various parent and child variables (e.g., parenting stress, parent emotional regulation, parent relationship satisfaction, child autism characteristics) and child externalizing behaviors, highlighting a potential target (i.e., parenting practices) for interventions to mitigate the impact of for example parenting stress on child behavior.

Mediating or moderating effects of child age were not explored among the included studies. Only one study explored the moderating effects of gender and found no effects. Age and gender were controlled for in some studies. Considering that research suggests that autistic females may use more compensatory behaviors (‘camouflaging’) (Bitsika & Sharpley, 2019), gender differences in behaviors may moderate the association between parenting practices and child externalizing behaviors and, therefore, requires further research. Research also suggests that some behaviors may be shown more often by younger autistic children (e.g., eating difficulties, sleeping, dependency, emotional regulation), and may be associated with higher levels of stress in for parents (Davis & Carter, 2008), which may impact associations with outcomes. This highlights the consideration of the effects of these variables in future autism research.

Across studies, there was variability in the way parenting practices and child behaviors were operationalized and measured. Only three studies used autism-specific measures to evaluate parenting practices, of which only one study used autism-specific measures to evaluate both parenting practices and child externalizing behaviors. The other studies used a range of different measures of parenting practices and child externalizing behaviors, which are validated for neurotypical families and not neurodivergent families and, therefore, not likely representative of the autism community (e.g., specific parenting practices may be beneficial for autistic children to provide structure and consistency). Further, six of 30 included studies used total scores of child behaviors (therefore, including a broader range of behaviors). More inclusive research with increased specificity (e.g., using subscale scores) and autism-specific measures/methodology will lead to improved generalizability and practical application of findings (e.g., identify specific intervention targets that aligns with the goals of the autistic community). The varying definitions and measures of child and parent variables lead to a lack of specificity on definitions of key variables and highlight the need for clear definitions for parent and child variables within the autism field. Further, it is also important to factor that not all discipline practices are a ‘negative’ parenting practice (Berliner et al., 2020).

Studies included in this review were based on self-report data on perceived parenting practices and parent-report data on child externalizing behaviors by parents, and thus, subject to bias (e.g., social desirability bias on rating ones’ own parenting approach; as well as stressed and/or depressed parents likely to over-report levels of child externalizing behaviors). Future researchers could consider the use of multi-informant data to reduce biases associated with self-reports (e.g., teacher) and/or use clinician observed data as well. Children’s perceptions of their behaviors could also be considered to gain a broader understanding of parenting practices relationships with externalizing behaviors.

The review also identified the variability in the definitions and measures used to investigate key constructs within the autism field (e.g., parenting, child behaviors, externalizing, and internalizing behaviors). Only three studies used autism-specific measures. Future research could use autism-specific measures (e.g., The Autism Symptoms Dimension Questionnaire, Developmental Behavior Checklist, Parenting Strategies Questionnaire) to ensure the measures are sensitive to differences in child externalizing behaviors (e.g., is social avoidance due to the profile of autism or other difficulties). Future research could also use autism-specific measures that captures differences in severity of the externalizing behaviors to reduce associated ceiling effects that measures designed for neurotypical populations may not capture, as well as identify other externalizing behaviors not commonly seen in neurotypical children (e.g., echolalia) (Adams et al., 2019). This also highlights the importance of further research on appropriate and universal terminology for key constructs for this community. This will help increase the robustness of the literature and consequently supports for this community.

Limitations

While this review addresses an important gap in the literature of synthesizing the quantitative research to date on the association between parenting practices and child externalizing behaviors a number of limitations are acknowledged. First, a choice was made a priori to focus on only quantitative articles. Qualitative studies and synthesis such as meta-synthesis may be valuable in future to provide in-depth understanding of particular behaviors (Robertson & Simmons, 2015), and important stakeholder perspectives (e.g., on the ‘why’). Second, only English language articles were included in this review, likely excluding several studies from non-English speaking countries/published in a different language, which may explain or at least contribute to the lack of diversity in participant ethnicity/race in reviewed studies. Future research could evaluate relationships between parenting practices and externalizing behaviors in autistic youth with more diverse populations, noting the homogeneity of the samples included in this review (86% of sample categorized as white). Third, to ensure manageable numbers, the search strategy did not include terms for parenting stress which may have led to under-identification of additional papers on moderators and mediators effects of parenting stress on the association between parenting practices and externalizing behaviors (considering the wealth of research on high levels of parenting stress in autistic parents, e.g., Costa et al., 2017; Dieleman et al., 2017; Frantz et al., 2018) which may be an important explicit focus in future reviews.

Clinical Implications and Conclusion

Based on the findings of this review, fostering mindful parenting practices and reducing parenting practices associated with higher levels of externalizing behavior [control dimension (i.e., negative control, psychological control, overreactivity) and authoritarian parenting styles (low warmth, high control)], among parents of autistic children has valuable potential for supports for parents and their children. Review findings reinforce that parenting is a complex concept with varied definitions and measured with a range measures, making it difficult to compare, contrast, synthesise, or replicate results. Therefore, using consistent definitions in the field along with measures specific to this population is highlighted as an important area for future research. For example, the Aberrant Behavior Scale—Irritability subscale (Aman et al., 1985) has been a gold standard measure for measuring behavioral problems, specifically—irritability, as well as broadly—tantrums, verbal meltdowns, negative mood, and self-harm in autistic children (Stoddard et al., 2020), as well as medical interventions for behavioral problems in autism (see review by Persico et al., 2021). Given that parents of autistic children experience poorer psychological health as compared to other parents (see review by Enea & Rusu, 2020) and may be at increased risk of developing mental health challenges (Beer et al., 2013), which may likely affect parenting practices and subsequently child externalizing behaviors, it is important to identify data specific to this community. Therefore, for clinical purposes, it would be important to use autism-specific measures, as using measures only validated for the neurotypical populations may impact findings by not accurately capturing data related to the autistic community, therefore, likely impacting interventions and supports for this community. Further research into parenting practices and child externalizing behaviors and the potential effects of parenting stress on this association is imperative to inform the development of supports for the autism community by targeting modifiable variables, to help increase overall well-being of both parents and their children.