Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Exploring why we learn from productive failure: insights from the cognitive and learning sciences

  • Invited Paper
  • Published:
Advances in Health Sciences Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Advances in Health Sciences Education (AHSE) has been at the forefront of the cognitive wave in health professions education for the past 25 years. One example is research on productive failure, a teaching strategy that asks learners to attempt to generate solutions to difficult problems before receiving instruction. This study compared the effectiveness of productive failure with indirect failure to further characterize the underpinning cognitive mechanisms of productive failure. Year one pharmacy students (N = 42) were randomly assigned to a productive failure or an indirect failure learning condition. The problem of estimating renal function based on serum creatinine was described to participants in the productive failure learning condition, who were then asked to generate a solution. Participants in the indirect failure condition learned about the same problem and were given incorrect solutions that other students had created, as well as the Cockcroft–Gault formula, and asked to compare and contrast the equations. Immediately thereafter all participants completed a series of tests designed to assess acquisition, application, and preparation for future learning (PFL). The tests were repeated after a 1-week delay. Participants in the productive failure condition outperformed those in the indirect failure condition, both on the immediate PFL assessment, and after a 1-week delay. These results emphasize the crucial role of generation in learning. When preparing novice students to learn new knowledge in the future, generating solutions to problems prior to instruction may be more effective than simply learning about someone else’s mistakes. Struggle and failure are most productive when experienced personally by a learner because it requires the learner to engage in generation, which deepens conceptual understanding.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bjork, R. A. (1994). Memory and metamemory considerations in the training of human beings. In J. Metcalfe & A. P. Shimamura (Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing (pp. 185–205). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2014). Making things hard on yourself, but in a good way: Creating desirable difficulties to enhance learning. In M. Gernsbacher & J. Pomerantz (Eds.), Psychology and the real world: Essays illustrating fundamental contributions to society (pp. 56–64). New York: Worth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R. A. (1975). Retrieval as a memory modifier: An interpretation of negative recency & related phenomena. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium (pp. 123–144). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R. A., & Bjork, E. L. (2019). Forgetting as the friend of learning: Implications for teaching and self-regulated learning. Advances in Physiology Education, 43(2), 164–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boshuizen, H. P. A., & Schmidt, H. G. (1992). On the role of biomedical knowledge in clinical reasoning by experts, intermediates and novices. Cognitive Science, 16(2), 153–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cleland, J., Varpio, L., Driessen, E., & Wilkinson, T. (2018). Lesson Learned: progressing knowledge by intelligently considering failures. Paper presented at the AMEE, Basel, Switzerland.

  • Cockcroft, D. W., & Gault, M. H. (1976). Prediction of creatinine clearance from serum creatinine. Nephron, 16, 31–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edmondson, A. (2011). Strategies of learning from failure. Harvard Business Review, 89, 48–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eva, K. W. (2018). Cognitive influences on complex performance assessment: Lessons from the interplay between medicine and psychology. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 7(2), 177–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eva, K. W., & Regehr, G. (2011). Exploring the divergence between self-assessment and self-monitoring. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 16(3), 311–329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatano, G., & Inagaki, K. (1986). Two courses of expertise. In H. W. Stevenson, H. E. Azuma, & K. E. Hakuta (Eds.), Child development and education in Japan (pp. 262–272). New York, NY: W H Freeman/Times Books/ Henry Holt & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kapur, M. (2008). Productive failure. Cognition and Instruction, 26, 379–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kapur, M. (2014a). Comparing learning from productive failure and vicarious failure. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 23(4), 651–677.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kapur, M. (2014b). Productive failure in learning math. Cognitive Science, 38, 1008–1022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kapur, M. (2016). Examining productive failure, productive success, unproductive failure, and unproductive success in Learning. Educational Psychologist, 51, 289–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kawamura, A., Harris, I., Thomas, K., Mema, B., & Mylopoulos, M. (2020). Exploring how pediatric residents develop adaptive expertise in communication: The importance of “shifts” in understanding patient and family perspectives. Academic Medicine, 95(7), 1066–1072.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work. Educational Psychologist, 41, 75–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klasen, J. M., & Lingard, L. A. (2019). Allowing failure for educational purposes in postgraduate clinical training: A narrative review. Medical Teacher, 41(11), 1263–1269. https://doi.org/10.1080/0142159X.2019.1630728.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mylopoulos, M., & Farhat, W. (2015). “I can do better”: Exploring purposeful improvement in daily clinical work. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 20(2), 371–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mylopoulos, M., Kulasegaram, K., & Woods, N. N. (2018). Developing the experts we need: Fostering adaptive expertise through education. Journal of Evaluation in Clinical Practice, 24(3), 674–677.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mylopoulos, M., Lohfeld, L., Norman, G. R., Dhaliwal, G., & Eva, K. W. (2012). Renowned physicians’ perceptions of expert diagnostic practice. Academic Medicine, 87(10), 1413–1417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mylopoulos, M., & Woods, N. N. (2014). Preparing medical students for future learning using basic science instruction. Medical Education, 48, 667–673.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, G. (2013). The third wave in health sciences education. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 18(3), 319–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, G., Sherbino, J., Dore, K., Wood, T., Young, M., Gaissmaier, W., & Monteiro, S. (2014). The etiology of diagnostic errors: A controlled trial of system 1 versus system 2 reasoning. Academic Medicine, 89(2), 277–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, G. R., Monteiro, S. D., Sherbino, J., Ilgen, J. S., Schmidt, H. G., & Mamede, S. (2017). The causes of errors in clinical reasoning: Cognitive biases, knowledge deficits, and dual process thinking. Academic Medicine, 92(1), 23–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Payne, D. G., Neely, J. H., & Burns, D. J. (1986). The generation effect: Further tests of the lexical activation hypothesis. Memory & Cognition, 14(3), 246–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reeve, J. (2002). Self-determination theory applied to educational settings. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 183–203). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosner, Z. A., Elman, J. A., & Shimamura, A. P. (2013). The generation effect: activating broad neural circuits during memory encoding. Cortex; A Journal DEVOTED to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 49(7), 1901–1909.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, H. G., & Rikers, R. M. (2007). How expertise develops in medicine: knowledge encapsulation and illness script formation. Medical Education, 41(12), 1133–1139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, R. A., & Bjork, R. A. (1992). New conceptualizations of practice: Common principles in three paradigms suggest new concepts for training. Psychological Science, 3, 207–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, D., & Martin, T. (2004). Inventing to prepare for future learning: The hidden efficiency of encouraging original student production in statistics instruction. Cognition and Instruction, 22, 129–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slamecka, N. J., & Graf, P. (1978). The generation effect: Delineation of a phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4(6), 592–604.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soderstrom, N. C., & Bjork, R. A. (2015). Learning versus performance: An integrative review. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(2), 176–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steenhof, N., Woods, N. N., Van Gerven, P. W. M., & Mylopoulos, M. (2019). Productive failure as an instructional approach to promote future learning. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 24(4), 739–749.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: effects on learning. Cognitive Science, 12(2), 257–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woods, N. N., Brooks, L. R., & Norman, G. R. (2007). It all make sense: Biomedical knowledge, causal connections and memory in the novice diagnostician. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 12(4), 405–415. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-006-9055-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young, M. (2019). The utility of failure: A taxonomy for research and scholarship. Perspectives on Medical Education, 8(6), 365–371. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40037-019-00551-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yudkowsky, R., Otaki, J., Lowenstein, T., Riddle, J., Nishigori, H., & Bordage, G. (2009). A hypothesis-driven physical examination learning and assessment procedure for medical students: Initial validity evidence. Medical Education, 43(8), 729–740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Naomi Steenhof.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Steenhof, N., Woods, N.N. & Mylopoulos, M. Exploring why we learn from productive failure: insights from the cognitive and learning sciences. Adv in Health Sci Educ 25, 1099–1106 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-020-10013-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-020-10013-y

Keywords

Navigation