Skip to main content
Log in

Visuospatial or verbal–spatial codes? The different effect of two secondary tasks on the power–space associations during a semantic categorizing task

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

A Correction to this article was published on 23 August 2023

This article has been updated

Abstract

The power–space associations have been extensively studied as a possible way to reveal the nature of concept representations, while the visuospatial and verbal–spatial codes are two primary explanations for the phenomenon. In two experiments, we imposed either a visuospatial or a verbal secondary task during the semantic categorizing of power words to examine their respective roles. The results showed that retaining a letter but not a location concurrently interfered with the power–space association. The results suggested that the verbal–spatial codes might play a more fundamental role than the visuospatial codes in the power–space associations during the semantic categorizing of power words.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy restrictions.

Change history

References

  • Anderson ML (2010) Neural reuse: a fundamental organizational principle of the brain. Behav Brain Sci 33:245–266

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou LW (1999) Perceptual symbol systems. Behav Brain Sci 22:577–609

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou LW (2008) Cognitive and neural contributions to understanding the conceptual system. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 17:91–95

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou LW (2016) On staying grounded and avoiding quixotic dead ends. Psychon Bull Rev 23:1122–1142

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Casasanto D (2014) Experiential origins of mental metaphors: language, culture, and the body. In: Landau M, Robinson MD, Meier BP (eds) The power of metaphor: examining its influence on social life. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, pp 249–268

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Chiao JY (2010) Neural basis of social status hierarchy across species. Curr Opin Neurobiol 20:803–809

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chiao JY, Bordeaux AR, Ambady N (2004) Mental representations of social status. Cognition 93:B49–B57

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chiao JY, Harada T, Oby ER, Li Z, Parrish T, Bridge DJ (2009) Neural representations of social status hierarchy in human inferior parietal cortex. Neuropsychologia 47:354–363

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dai Q, Zhu L (2018) Verbal-spatial and visuospatial coding of power-space interactions. Conscious Cogn 63:151–160

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene S, Bossini S, Giraux P (1993) The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. J Exp Psychol Gen 122:371–396

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene S, Dupoux E, Mehler J (1990) Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 16:626–641

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene S, Piazza M, Pinel P, Cohen L (2003) Three parietal circuits for number processing. Cogn Neuropsychol 20:487–506

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A (2007) G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods 39(2):175–191

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Galinsky AD, Gruenfeld DH, Magee JC (2003) From power to action. J Pers Soc Psychol 85:453–466

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Georges C, Schiltz C, Hoffmann D (2015) Task instructions determine the visuospatial and verbal–spatial nature of number–space associations. Quart J Exp Psychol 68:1895–1909

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gevers W, Santens S, Dhooge E, Chen Q, Van den Bossche L, Fias W, Verguts T (2010) Verbal-spatial and visuospatial coding of number-space interactions. J Exp Psychol-General 139:180–190

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giessner SR, Schubert TW (2007) High in the hierarchy: how vertical location and judgments of leaders’ power are interrelated. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 104:30–44

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg AM (1997) What memory is for. Behav Brain Sci 20:1–55

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herrera A, Macizo P, Semenza C (2008) The role of working memory in the association between number magnitude and space. Acta Physiol (Oxf) 128:225–237

    Google Scholar 

  • Hommel B, Musseler J, Aschersleben G, Prinz W (2001) The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behav Brain Sci 24:849–937

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hubbard EM, Piazza M, Pinel P, Dehaene S (2005) Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:435–448

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Institute of Language Teaching, Beijing Language College (1986) Word frequency dictionary of modern Chinese. Beijing Language College Press, Beijing ((Chinese))

    Google Scholar 

  • Jiang TJ, Sun LN, Zhu L (2015) The influence of vertical motor responses on explicit and incidental processing of power words. Conscious Cogn 34:33–42

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jiang TJ, Zhu L (2015) Is power-space a continuum? Distance effect during power judgments. Conscious Cogn 37:8–15

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keltner D, Gruenfeld DH, Anderson C (2003) Power, approach, and inhibition. Psychol Rev 110:265–284

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kemmerer D (2015) Are the motor features of verb meanings represented in the precentral motor cortices? Yes, but within the context of a flexible, multilevel architecture for conceptual knowledge. Psychon Bull Rev 22:1068–1075

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff G (1987) Women, fire, and dangerous things. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff G, Johnson M (1980) Metaphors we live by. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL

    Google Scholar 

  • Longe O, Randall B, Stamatakis EA, Tyler LK (2007) Grammatical categories in the brain: the role of morphological structure. Cereb Cortex 17:1812–1820

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lu L, Schubert T, Zhu L (2017) The spatial representation of power in children. Cogn Process 18:375–385

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mahon BZ, Caramazza A (2008) A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content. J Physiol-Paris 102:59–70

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mason M, Magee JC, Fiske ST (2014) Neural substrates of social status inference: roles of medial prefrontal cortex and superior temporal sulcus. J Cogn Neurosci 26:1131–1140

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Meier BP, Dionne S (2009) Downright sexy: verticality, implicit power, and perceived physical attractiveness. Soc Cogn 27:883–892

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meier BP, Robinson MD (2004) Why the sunny side is up: associations between affect and vertical position. Psychol Sci 15:243–247

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neumann R, Strack F (2000) Approach and avoidance: the influence of proprioceptive and exteroceptive cues on encoding of affective information. J Pers Soc Psychol 79:39–48

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paivio A (1986) Mental representations: a dual coding approach. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio A (1991) Dual coding theory: retrospect and current status. Can J Psychol 45:255–287

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perani D, Cappa SF, Schnur T, Tettamanti M, Collina S, Rosa MM, Fazio F (1999) The neural correlates of verbal and noun processing. A PET Study Brain 122:2337–2344

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schubert TW (2005) Your highness: vertical positions as perceptual symbols of power. J Pers Soc Psychol 89:1–21

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz B, Tesser A, Powell E (1982) Dominance cues in nonverbal behavior. Soc Psychol Quart 45:114–120

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suitner C, Schubert TW (2018) Grounding social cognition in space. In: Hubbard T (ed) Spatial biases in perception and cognition. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 336–349

  • Tang F, Zhu M, Zhang H, Zhu L (2018) Development of spatial representation of power in children. J Exp Child Psychol 173:239–249

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasino B, Rumiati RI (2013) At the mercy of strategies: The role of motor representations in language understanding. Front Psychol, 4.

  • van Dijck JP, Gevers W, Fias W (2009) Numbers are associated with different types of spatial information depending on the task. Cognition 113:248–253

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yang H, Nick Reid J, Katz AN, Li D (2021) The embodiment of power as forward/backward movement in Chinese and English speakers. Metaphor Symb 36:181–193

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yee E, Thompson-Schill SL (2016) Putting concepts into context. Psychon Bull Rev 23:1015–1027

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zanolie K, van Dantzig S, Boot I, Wijnen J, Schubert TW, Giessner SR, Pecher D (2012) Mighty metaphors: behavioral and ERP evidence that power shifts attention on a vertical dimension. Brain Cogn 78:50–58

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang P, Schubert TW, Zhu L (2019) The effect of secondary task on the association between power and space. Soc Cogn 37:1–17

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by National Key R&D Program of China (2022ZD0116403), the research fund of the School of Social Development and Public Policy at Fudan University and the Humanities and Social Sciences Youth Integration Innovation Team of Fudan University.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lei Zhu.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

Ethical Statement

The study is approved by the IRB of School of Social Development and Public Policy at Fudan University (FDU-SSDPP-IRB-2020-2-002).

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Editors: Joanna Ganczarek (Pedagogical University of Cracow), Hamad Al-Azary (Lawrence Technological University); Reviewers: Huilan Yang (Zhejiang Gongshang University) and a second researcher who prefers to remain anonymous.

Appendix

Appendix

Words represent human used in the experiments.

Word

In English

Valence rating

Power rating

Word frequency

霸主

Overlord

4.75

6.75

1

君主

Sovereign

5.42

6.33

12

首领

Leader

4.92

5.50

8

校长

Principal

4.33

4.17

52

主人

Host

3.50

4.25

83

主任

Director

3.25

3.33

177

总理

Prime Minister

4.75

5.42

222

总统

President

4.92

6.17

21

官员

Officer

2.92

4.50

8

国王

King

4.50

5.83

52

将军

General

5.17

5.42

91

教练

Coach

4.08

3.17

34

经理

Manager

3.92

3.75

131

老板

Boss

3.67

4.58

117

统治者

Governor

3.42

6.50

1

政治家

Politician

2.83

4.33

1

  1. Note: the unit of word frequency is 1/1310000

Word

In English

Valence rating

Power rating

Word frequency

孩子

Child

5.92

1.92

1026

农夫

Plowman

4.58

3.33

9

农民

Farmer

4.33

3.33

290

仆人

Servant

3.25

2.08

6

实习生

Intern

4.08

2.50

1

士兵

Soldier

4.50

4.08

45

水手

Sailor

5.00

3.83

16

学生

Student

5.00

3.00

424

保姆

Nurserymaid

3.67

2.58

3

病人

Sick person

2.83

1.75

81

孤儿

Orphan

3.25

1.67

6

奴隶

Slave

2.17

1.75

73

随从

Attendant

3.08

2.42

3

学徒

Apprentice

3.67

2.58

17

丫头

Female Servant

4.83

2.00

56

婴儿

Baby

6.17

1.58

35

  1. Note: the unit of word frequency is 1/1310000

Words represent animals used in the experiments.

Word

In English

Valence rating

Power rating

Word frequency

狮子

Lion

5.33

6.42

21

非洲狮

Africa lion

5.08

6.50

1

猎豹

Cheetah

5.08

5.67

1

老虎

Tiger

4.92

6.25

74

东北虎

Northeast Tiger

5.08

6.42

1

黑熊

Black bear

3.83

5.92

1

野熊

Wild bear

3.92

6.08

1

大象

Elephant

5.83

5.67

9

藏獒

Tibetan Mastiff

3.92

5.50

1

蟒蛇

Boa

2.50

5.33

1

巨蟒

Python

2.42

5.83

1

大白鲨

Great white shark

4.17

5.83

1

鲨鱼

Shark

4.00

5.42

1

巨鲸

Gigantic whale

5.50

5.08

1

蓝鲸

Blue whale

5.67

4.75

7

鳄鱼

Crocodile

3.67

5.83

14

  1. Note: the unit of word frequency is 1/1310000

Word

In English

Valence rating

Power rating

Word frequency

小鸡

Chick

4.75

1.25

1

鸭子

Duck

4.50

1.50

20

兔子

Rabbit

5.58

1.42

13

小猫

Kitten

5.58

1.58

1

吉娃娃

Chihuahua

4.83

1.42

1

金鱼

Goldfish

4.75

1.08

30

蜗牛

Snail

4.00

1.00

11

蝌蚪

Tadpole

4.00

1.00

1

松鼠

Squirrel

5.92

1.58

17

小白鼠

Mice

4.75

1.17

1

蜻蜓

Dragonfly

4.08

1.08

21

蝴蝶

Butterfly

4.50

1.08

35

蚂蚁

Ant

3.50

1.33

40

毛毛虫

Caterpillar

1.58

1.33

1

蚯蚓

Earthworm

2.50

1.25

16

麻雀

Sparrow

3.83

1.50

19

  1. Note: the unit of word frequency is 1/1310000

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sai, X., Wu, X., Li, X. et al. Visuospatial or verbal–spatial codes? The different effect of two secondary tasks on the power–space associations during a semantic categorizing task. Cogn Process 24, 481–493 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-023-01149-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-023-01149-x

Keywords

Navigation