Skip to main content
Log in

Frequency of removal movements during social versus self-grooming among wild chimpanzees

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Primates Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Grooming was observed in 11 wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in Mahale, Tanzania, and the number of removal and stroke movements and grooming duration were recorded. Removal movements were more frequent during social grooming than during self-grooming. Chimpanzees used one or both hands for grooming, and grooming using both hands was more efficient for removing small objects. Due to physical constraints, self-grooming of the arms was almost always done using only one hand. The removal movement frequency during arm grooming was lower when self-grooming than when grooming another. They were more likely to use both hands during grooming another than during self-grooming, and fewer physical constraints during social grooming enabled a higher level of hygienic grooming.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barton R (1985) Grooming site preferences in primates and their functional implications. Int J Primatol 6:519–532

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blanc G, Woodward TE (1945) The infection of Pedicinus albidus Rudow, the maggot louse, on typhus carrying monkeys (Macacus sylvanus). Am J Trop Med 25:33–34

    Google Scholar 

  • Boccia ML (1983) A functional analysis of social grooming patterns through direct comparison with self-grooming in rhesus monkeys. Int J Primatol 4:399–418

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boccia ML, Reite M, Laudenslager M (1989) On the physiology of grooming in a pigtail macaque. Physiol Behav 45:667–670

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Borries C (1992) Grooming site preferences in female langurs (Presbytis entellus). Int J Primatol 13:19–32

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brain C (1992) Deaths in a desert baboon troop. Int J Primatol 13:593–599

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar RIM (1991) Functional significance of social grooming in primates. Folia Primatol 57:121–131

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freeland WJ (1981) Functional aspects of primate grooming. Ohio Acad Sci 81:173–177

    Google Scholar 

  • Furuya Y (1957) Grooming behavior in wild Japanese monkeys. Primates 1:47–68

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodall J (1968) The behaviour of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve. Anim Behav Monogr 1:161–311

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins M, Barash DP (1976) Grooming in primates: implications for its utilitarian function. Primates 17:145–150

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kettle DS (1990) Medical and veterinary entomology. CAB International, Oxon

  • Kuhn HJ (1968) Parasites and the phylogeny of the catarrhine primates. In: Chiarelli B (ed) Taxonomy and phylogeny of Old World primates with references to the origin of man. Rosenberg and Sellier, Torino, pp 187–195

    Google Scholar 

  • Marchant LF, McGrew WC (1996) Laterality of limb function in wild chimpanzees of Gombe National Park: comprehensive study of spontaneous activities. J Hum Evol 30:427–443

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myers BJ, Kuntz RE (1972) A checklist of parasites and commensals reported for the chimpanzee (Pan). Primates 13:433–471

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nakamura M (2003) ‘Gatherings’ of social grooming among wild chimpanzees: implications for evolution of sociality. J Hum Evol 44:59–71

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nishida T (1990) A quarter century of research in the Mahale Mountains: an overview. In: Nishida T (ed) The chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains. University of Tokyo Press, Tokyo, pp 3–35

    Google Scholar 

  • Nishida T, Corp N, Hamai M, Hasegawa T, Hiraiwa-Hasegawa M, Hosaka K, Hunt KD, Itoh N, Kawanaka K, Matsumoto-Oda A, Mitani JC, Nakamura M, Norikoshi K, Sakamaki T, Turner L, Uehara S, Zamma K (2003) Demography, female life history, and reproductive profiles among the chimpanzees of Mahale. Am J Primatol 59:99–121

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • R Development Core Team (2010) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna. ISBN 3-900051-07-0. http://www.R-project.org

  • Reichard U, Sommer V (1994) Grooming site preferences in wild white-handed gibbons (Hylobates lar). Primates 35:369–374

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schino G, Scucchi S, Maestripieri D, Turillazzi PG (1988) Allogrooming as a tension-reduction mechanism: a behavioral approach. Am J Primatol 16:43–50

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth RM (1977) A model of social grooming among adult female monkeys. J Theor Biol 65:671–698

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth RM (1980) The distribution of grooming and related behaviours among adult female vervet monkeys. Anim Behav 28:798–813

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tanaka I, Takefushi H (1993) Elimination of external parasites (lice) is the primary function of grooming in free-ranging Japanese macaques. Anthropol Sci 101:187–193

    Google Scholar 

  • Terry RL (1970) Primate grooming as a tension reduction mechanism. J Psychol 76:129–136

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • T-W-Fiennes RN (1972) Ectoparasites and vectors. In: T-W-Fiennes RN (ed) Pathology of simian primates, part II. Karger, Basel, pp 158–176

    Google Scholar 

  • Zamma K (2002a) Grooming site preferences determined by lice infection among Japanese macaques in Arashiyama. Primates 43:41–49

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zamma K (2002b) Leaf-grooming by a wild chimpanzee in Mahale. Primates 43:87–90

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zamma K (2006) A louse egg left on a leaf. Pan Afr News 13:8–10

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This study was financially supported by grants from the COE program of Monbusho (#10CE2005 to O. Takenaka) and the Leakey Foundation (to T. Nishida), and by a MEXT Grant-in-Aid for JSPS Fellows (15004835 to K. Zamma). I am grateful to TAWIRI, COSTECH, and TANAPA for permission to work in Mahale. I also thank the MMWRC and MMNP for logistic support. I am also grateful to T. Nishida, J. Yamagiwa, N. Koyama, M. Imafuku, and M. Nakatsukasa for their comments and advice about the manuscript. I also express my gratitude to D.A. Collins, H. Ihobe, S. Suzuki, I. Tanaka, Y. Muroyama, Y. Takenoshita, N. Itoh, and members of Human Evolution Studies at Kyoto University for their advice, and to H.Y. Kayumbo, T. Nemoto, N. Corp, and T. Sakamaki for their help at the field station. I also acknowledge the assistance of K. Athmani and H. Bunengwa and the entire staff of the Mahale Mountains Chimpanzee Research Project for observing chimpanzees in the formidable bush.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Koichiro Zamma.

About this article

Cite this article

Zamma, K. Frequency of removal movements during social versus self-grooming among wild chimpanzees. Primates 52, 323–328 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-011-0267-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-011-0267-1

Keywords

Navigation