Skip to main content
Log in

Disturbance cues function as a background risk cue but not as an associative learning cue in tadpoles

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Animal Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Chemical information has an important role in the sensory ecology of aquatic species. For aquatic prey, chemical cues are a vital source of information related to predator avoidance and risk assessment. For instance, alarm cues are released by prey that have been injured by predators. In addition to providing accurate information about current risk, repeated exposure to alarm cues can elicit a fear response to novel stimuli (neophobia) in prey. Another source of chemical information is disturbance cues, released by prey that have been disturbed or harassed (but not injured) by a predator. While disturbance cues have received much less attention than alarm cues, they appear to be useful as an early warning signal of predation risk and have the potential to be used as a priming cue for learning. In this study, we used wood frog (Lithobates sylvaticus) tadpoles to test whether repeated exposure to disturbance cues during the embryonic stage can induce neophobic behaviour. Three weeks following repeated exposure to disturbance cues, tadpoles showed reduced activity when exposed to a novel odour, but they no longer displayed an antipredator response to disturbance cues. In a second experiment, we found that tadpoles failed to learn that a novel odour was dangerous following a pairing with disturbance cues, whereas alarm cues facilitated such learning. Our results add to the growing body of information about disturbance cues and provide evidence of their function as an embryonic risk cue but not as an associative learning cue.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Availability of data and material

All data from this study are included as supplementary information.

Code availability

Not applicable.

References

  • Abreu MS, Giacomini AC, Gusso D, Koakoski G, Oliveira TA, Marqueze A, Barreto RE, Barcellos LJ (2016) Behavioral responses of zebrafish depend on the type of threatening chemical cues. J Comp Physiol 202:895–901

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bairos-Novak KR, Mitchell MD, Crane AL, Chivers DP, Ferrari MCO (2017) Trust thy neighbour in times of trouble: background risk alters how tadpoles release and respond to disturbance cues. Proc R Soc B 284:20171465

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bairos-Novak KR, Crane AL, Chivers DP, Ferrari MCO (2019) Better the devil you know? How familiarity and kinship affect prey responses to disturbance cues. Behav Ecol 30:446–454

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bannier F, Tebbich S, Taborsky B (2017) Early experience affects learning performance and neophobia in a cooperatively breeding cichlid. Ethology 123:712–723

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berejikian BA, Tezak EP, LaRae AL (2003) Innate and enhanced predator recognition in hatchery-reared chinook salmon. Environ Biol Fishes 67:241–251

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bett NN, Hinch SG, Yun SS (2016) Behavioural responses of Pacific salmon to chemical disturbance cues during the spawning migration. Behav Process 132:76–84

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brönmark C, Hansson LA (2000) Chemical communication in aquatic systems: an introduction. Oikos 88:103–109

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown GE (2003) Learning about danger: chemical alarm cues and local risk assessment in prey fishes. Fish Fish 43:227–234

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown GE, Jackson CD, Malka PH, Jacques É, Couturier MA (2012) Disturbance cues in freshwater prey fishes: Does urea function as an ‘early warning cue’ in juvenile convict cichlids and rainbow trout? Curr Zool 58:250–259

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brown GE, Ferrari MCO, Elvidge CK, Ramnarine I, Chivers DP (2013) Phenotypically plastic neophobia: a response to variable predation risk. Proc R Soc B 280:20122712

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brown GE, Demers EE, Joyce BJ, Ferrari MCO, Chivers DP (2015) Retention of neophobic predator recognition in juvenile convict cichlids: effects of background risk and recent experience. Anim Cogn 18:1331–1338

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown GE, Jackson CD, Joyce BJ, Chivers DP, Ferrari MCO (2016) Risk-induced neophobia: does sensory modality matter? Anim Cogn 19:1143–1150

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chivers DP, Ferrari MC (2013) Tadpole antipredator responses change over time: what is the role of learning and generalization? Behav Ecol 24:1114–1121

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chivers DP, Mirza RS, Bryer PJ, Kiesecker JM (2001) Threat-sensitive predator avoidance by slimy sculpins: understanding the importance of visual versus chemical information. Can J Zool 79:867–873

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chivers DP, Brown GE, Ferrari MCO (2012) The evolution of alarm substances and disturbance cues in aquatic animals. In: Brönmark C, Hansson LA (eds) Chemical ecology in aquatic systems. Oxford Publisher Print, New York, pp 127–139

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Crane AL, Ferrari MC (2017a) Evidence for risk extrapolation in decision making by tadpoles. Sci Rep 7:1–7

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Crane AL, Ferrari MC (2017b) Patterns of predator neophobia: a meta-analytic review. Proc Royal Soc B 284:20170583

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crane AL, Mathis A (2013) Learning about danger by young hellbenders (Cryptobranchus alleganiensis): are antipredator strategies ontogenetically plastic? Amphibia-Reptilia 34:119–124

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crane AL, Helton EJ, Ferrari MCO, Mathis A (2018) Learning to find food: evidence for embryonic sensitization and juvenile social learning in a salamander. Anim Behav 142:199–206

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crane AL, Brown GE, Chivers DP, Ferrari MCO (2020a) An ecological framework of neophobia: from cells to organisms to populations. Biol Rev 95:218–231

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crane AL, Feyten LE, Ramnarine IW, Brown GE (2020b) Temporally variable predation risk and fear retention in Trinidadian guppies. Behav Ecol 31:1084–1090

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crane AL, Bairos-Novak KR, Goldman JA, Brown GE (2021) Chemical disturbance cues in aquatic systems: a review and prospectus. Ecol Monogr. https://doi.org/10.1002/ecm.1487

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dill LM (1987) Animal decision making and its ecological consequences: the future of aquatic ecology and behaviour. Can J Zool 65:803–811

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari MCO, Chivers DP (2008) Latent inhibition of predator recognition by embryonic amphibians. Biol Lett 5:160–162

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari MCO, Gonzalo A, Messier F, Chivers DP (2007a) Generalization of learned predator recognition: an experimental test and framework for future studies. Proc R Soc B 274:1853–1859

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari MCO, Messier F, Chivers DP (2007b) Degradation of chemical alarm cues under natural conditions: risk assessment by larval woodfrogs. Chemoecology 17:263–266

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari MCO, Vavrek MA, Elvidge CK, Fridman B, Chivers DP, Brown GE (2008) Sensory complementation and the acquisition of predator recognition by salmonid fishes. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 63:113–121

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari MC, Brown GE, Messier F, Chivers DP (2009) Threat-sensitive generalization of predator recognition by larval amphibians. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 63:1369–1375

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari MCO, Wisenden BD, Chivers DP (2010) Chemical ecology of predator–prey interactions in aquatic ecosystems: a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 88:698–724

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari MC, Crane AL, Brown GE, Chivers DP (2015a) Getting ready for invasions: can background level of risk predict the ability of naive prey to survive novel predators? Sci Rep 5:1–7

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari MCO, McCormick MI, Meekan MG, Chivers DP (2015b) Background level of risk and the survival of predator-naïve prey: can neophobia compensate for predator naivety in juvenile coral reef fishes? Proc R Soc B 282:20142197

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari MC, Horn ME, Chivers DP (2019) Cognitive resonance: when information carry-over constrains cognitive plasticity. Funct Ecol 33:703–711

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fraker ME, Hu F, Cuddapah V, McCollum SA, Relyea RA, Hempel J, Denver RJ (2009) Characterization of an alarm pheromone secreted by amphibian tadpoles that induces behavioral inhibition and suppression of the neuroendocrine stress axis. Horm Behav 55:520–529

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Glaudas X, Winne CT, Fedewa LA (2006) Ontogeny of anti-predator behavioral habituation in cottonmouths (Agkistrodon piscivorus). Ethology 112:608–615

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gonzalo A, López P, Martín J (2010) Risk level of chemical cues determines retention of recognition of new predators in Iberian green frog tadpoles. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 64:1117–1123

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gosner KL (1960) A simplified table for staging anuran embryos and larvae with notes on identification. Herpetologica 16:183–190

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin AS (2004) Social learning about predators: a review and prospectus. Anim Learn Behav 32:131–140

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkins LA, Magurran AE, Armstrong JD (2008) Ontogenetic learning of predator recognition in hatchery-reared Atlantic salmon, Salmo salar. Anim Behav 75:1663–1671

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hazlett BA (1985) Disturbance pheromones in the crayfish Orconectes virilis. J Chem Ecol 11:1695–1711

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Helfman GS (1989) Threat-sensitive predator avoidance in damselfish-trumpetfish interactions. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 24:47–58

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hemmi JM, Merkle T (2009) High stimulus specificity characterizes anti-predator habituation under natural conditions. Proc R Soc B 276:4381–4388

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Horn ME, Chivers DP (2021) Embryonic exposure to predation risk and hatch time variation in fathead minnows. PLoS ONE 16:e0255961

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ireland DH, Wirsing AJ, Murray DL (2007) Phenotypically plastic responses of green frog embryos to conflicting predation risk. Oecologia 152:162–168

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson DD, Blumstein DT, Fowler JH, Haselton MG (2013) The evolution of error: error management, cognitive constraints, and adaptive decision-making biases. Trends Ecol Evol 28:474–481

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jordão LC (2004) Disturbance chemical cues determine changes in spatial occupation by the convict cichlid Archocentrus nigrofasciatus. Behav Process 67:453–459

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jordão L, Volpato G (2000) Chemical transfer of warning information in non-injured fish. Behaviour 137:681–690

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelley JL, Magurran AE (2003) Learned predator recognition and antipredator responses in fishes. Fish Fish 4:216–226

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kiesecker JM, Chivers DP, Marco A, Quilchano C, Anderson MT, Blaustein AR (1999) Identification of a disturbance signal in larval red-legged frogs, Rana aurora. Anim Behav 57:1295–1300

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kusch RC, Mirza RS, Chivers DP (2004) Making sense of predator scents: investigating the sophistication of predator assessment abilities of fathead minnows. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 55:551–555

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lima SL, Bednekoff PA (1999) Temporal variation in danger drives antipredator behavior: the predation risk allocation hypothesis. Am Nat 153:649–659

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lima SL, Dill LM (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 68:619–640

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lönnstedt OM, McCormick MI (2011) Chemical alarm cues inform prey of predation threat: the importance of ontogeny and concentration in a coral reef fish. Anim Behav 82:213–218

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathis A, Crane AL (2017) Chemoreception. In: Call J, Burghardt GM, Pepperberg IM, Snowdon CT, Zentall T (eds) APA handbook of comparative psychology: perception, learning, and cognition 2. American Psychological Association, Washington, pp 69–87

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Mathis A, Smith RJ (1993) Fathead minnows, Pimephales promelas, learn to recognize northern pike, Esox lucius, as predators on the basis of chemical stimuli from minnows in the pike’s diet. Anim Behav 46:645–656

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathis A, Ferrari MCO, Windel N, Messier F, Chivers DP (2008) Learning by embryos and the ghost of predation future. Proc R Soc B 275:2603–2607

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mery F, Burns JG (2010) Behavioural plasticity: an interaction between evolution and experience. Evol Ecol 24:571–583

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mineka S, Cook M (1993) Mechanisms involved in the observational conditioning of fear. J Exp Psychol Gen 122:23–38

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mirza RS, Chivers DP (2000) Predator-recognition training enhances survival of brook trout: evidence from laboratory and field-enclosure studies. Can J Zool 78:2198–2208

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mirza R, Chivers D (2002) Behavioural responses to conspecific disturbance chemicals enhance survival of juvenile brook charr, Salvelinus fontinalis, during encounters with predators. Behaviour 139:1099–1109

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mirza RS, Ferrari MCO, Kiesecker JM, Chivers DP (2006) Responses of American toad tadpoles to predation cues: behavioural response thresholds, threat-sensitivity and acquired predation recognition. Behaviour 143:877–889

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell MD, Chivers DP, Brown GE, Ferrari MC (2016) Living on the edge: how does environmental risk affect the behavioural and cognitive ecology of prey? Anim Behav 115:185–192

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nishizaki MT, Ackerman JD (2005) A secondary chemical cue facilitates juvenile-adult postsettlement associations in red sea urchins. Limnol Oceanogr 50:354–362

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orizaola G, Brana F (2005) Plasticity in newt metamorphosis: the effect of predation at embryonic and larval stages. Freshw Biol 50:438–446

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov IP (1927) Conditioned reflexes. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Polo-Cavia N, Gomez-Mestre I (2014) Learned recognition of introduced predators determines survival of tadpole prey. Funct Ecol 28:432–439

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raderschall CA, Magrath RD, Hemmi JM (2011) Habituation under natural conditions: model predators are distinguished by approach direction. J Exp Biol 214:4209–4216

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rankin CH, Abrams T, Barry RJ, Bhatnagar S, Clayton DF, Colombo J, Coppola G, Geyer MA, Glanzman DL, Marsland S, McSweeney FK (2009) Habituation revisited: an updated and revised description of the behavioral characteristics of habituation. Neurobiol Learn Mem 92:135–138

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vavrek MA, Brown GE (2009) Threat-sensitive responses to disturbance cues in juvenile convict cichlids and rainbow trout. Ann Zool Fenn 46:171–180

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vavrek MA, Elvidge CK, DeCaire R, Belland B, Jackson CD, Brown GE (2008) Disturbance cues in freshwater prey fishes: do juvenile convict cichlids and rainbow trout respond to ammonium as an ‘early warning’ signal? Chemoecology 18:255–261

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wisenden BD (2000) Olfactory assessment of predation risk in the aquatic environment. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B 355:1205–1208

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wisenden BD, Chivers DP, Smith RJF (1995) Early warning in the predation sequence: a disturbance pheromone in Iowa darters (Etheostoma exile). J Chem Ecol 21:1469–1480

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhao X, Ferrari MC, Chivers DP (2006) Threat-sensitive learning of predator odours by a prey fish. Behaviour 1:1103–1121

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

Funding for this work was provided by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada and the Mexican National Council for Science and Technology (CONACYT; Grant number: 452400).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

IAERH, ALC, and MCOF designed the study. IAERH and ALC conducted the experiments and analyzed the data. All authors contributed to writing the manuscript, have approved it for publication and agree to be held accountable for its content.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ita A. E. Rivera-Hernández.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Ethics approval

The field collection of the wood frog egg clutches was approved by the Saskatchewan Ministry of Environment (Scientific Research Permit #18FW071). This study was conducted at the University of Saskatchewan and was approved by the University’s Animal Research Ethics Board (protocol #20060014).

Consent for publication

All authors consent to publication.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (XLSX 45 KB)

Supplementary file2 (DOCX 20 KB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rivera-Hernández, I.A.E., Crane, A.L., Pollock, M.S. et al. Disturbance cues function as a background risk cue but not as an associative learning cue in tadpoles. Anim Cogn 25, 881–889 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-022-01599-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-022-01599-4

Keywords

Navigation